Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 November 2012
I was running in the Botanical Gardens, while planning my day. It was a frosty, quiet, and beautiful day. I was running lost in my thoughts – and suddenly I ran over some slippery, frozen puddles. I suddenly remembered another run, also on a frosty day. I had passed another runner, with long, ruffled hair, and a big smile, who yelled: “Take care on the ice!”
Autobiographical memories often arise involuntarily, that is, with no preceding retrieval effort, as illustrated by the example. However, most research on autobiographical/episodic memory still works under the assumption that conscious recollections of past events normally require a goal-directed and controlled (voluntary) retrieval process. For example, in his seminal book, Tulving (1983) argued that “access to, or actualization from, the episodic memory system tends to be deliberate and usually requires conscious effort” (p. 46). Mandler (1985) observed that episodic retrieval in general is “deliberate and consciously accessed” (p. 94), while acknowledging that “much of everyday memory experiences are in fact non-deliberate” (pp. 102–103). More recently, Davachi and Dobbins (2008) stated, “Occasionally, a memory will just pop into one’s head wholly uninvited. Although we have all had such involuntary recollections, they are arguably not the norm. . .” (p. 115). Here I am going to challenge this view by arguing that involuntary recollections are a basic mode of remembering.
A basic mode of remembering
On the first pages of his groundbreaking book on memory, Ebbinghaus (1885) identified three basic modes of remembering, a voluntary conscious mode, an involuntary conscious mode, and a non-conscious mode. However, since Ebbinghaus, modern cognitive psychologists have generally ignored the category of involuntary conscious memories (see Berntsen, 2009; Hintzman, 2011, for reviews). In contrast, involuntary conscious memories have received substantial attention in the field of clinical psychology, but almost always in relation to emotionally stressful experiences or psychological disorders (e.g., Brewin, Dalgleish, and Joseph, 1996; Ehlers, Hackmann, and Michael, 2004).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.