Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 November 2012
The past isn’t dead; it isn’t even past.
– William FaulknerHow memories are consolidated is a problem that is at the core of memory research. Without consolidation, memories are fleeting; distraction, interference, noxious agents, drugs, brain damage, or the mere passage of time will disrupt and eliminate them. Consolidation is the time and experience-dependent process by which newly acquired memories become stabilized so that they are relatively immune to disruption by all these influences. Not surprisingly, therefore, consolidation has been a topic of investigation since the beginning of the scientific study of memory. In this chapter, I will focus on the role that autobiographical memory has played in our understanding of the neural mechanisms implicated in consolidation.
The turn of the twentieth century
Autobiographical memory played a significant role in establishing that brain damage or trauma can disrupt consolidation, and continues to play this role during bedside investigations of memory loss. Patients are asked routinely if they know where they are, what brought them to this location, what they did recently, and what they remember about their more remote past, such as the schools they attended, the friends they had, the jobs they held, and so on. They also are asked questions regarding general knowledge, such as who is the current mayor or head of the country, and about news events from the recent and remote past. Based on such anecdotal evidence, investigators dating back to the end of the nineteenth century concluded that recent memories are more vulnerable to disruption than remote memories, presumably because the latter had an opportunity to become consolidated whereas the former did not (Ribot, 1882). The term “consolidation” was coined by Müller and Pilzecker (1900) to account for the results of their behavioral, laboratory-based experiments in which they measured the time it took newly learned memories to become resistant to disruption by a subsequent, to-be-remembered event. By 1904, Burnham concluded:
The fixing [consolidation] of an impression depends on a physiological process. It takes time for an impression to become so fixed that it can be reproduced after a long interval; for it to become part of the permanent store of memory considerable time may be necessary. This we may suppose is not merely a process of making a permanent impression upon the nerve cells, but also a process of association, of organization of the new impressions with the old ones.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.