Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T06:26:49.055Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Kuhn on Concepts and Categorization

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2009

Peter Barker
Affiliation:
Professor of History of Science, The University of Oklahoma
Xiang Chen
Affiliation:
Associate Professor, California Lutheran University
Hanne Andersen
Affiliation:
Assistant Professor in the Department of Medical Philosophy and Clinical Theory, University of Copenhagen
Thomas Nickles
Affiliation:
University of Nevada, Reno
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

In Kuhn's account of the history of science, the nature of concepts and conceptual change looms large. Kuhn found little to admire in contemporary philosophical accounts of science, and he also found himself at odds with the philosophical community on the theory of concepts. Consequently, in the course of developing his philosophical account of science, he was also obliged to articulate a theory of concepts. One of the central ideas of his account, incommensurability, originated as a thesis about concepts. As his account matured, Kuhn came to formulate incommensurability as a thesis about taxonomies. The issue of categorization therefore emerges immediately from his account, with his theory of concepts providing the basis for the conceptual structures that he calls “kind hierarchies.”

Kuhn's theory is not without precedent. It builds on the work of Wittgenstein and also reflects Kuhn's early and profound exposure to Kant. In a revealing interview near the end of his life Kuhn said simply, “I am a Kantian with movable categories” (Baltas, Gavroglu, and Kindi 2000, p. 264). Provided that the categories are understood as Wittgensteinean family resemblance concepts, this is a valuable summary. As his philosophy of science developed, Kuhn focused increasingly on the nature of scientific concepts, and his account of concepts gradually became the foundation from which he sought to vindicate his earlier claims on the development and change of scientific knowledge.

Type
Chapter
Information
Thomas Kuhn , pp. 212 - 245
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen, H. 1996. “Categorization, Anomalies and the Discovery of Nuclear Fission.” Studies in the History and Philosophy of Modern Physics 27: 463–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, H. 2000a. “Learning by Ostension: Thomas Kuhn on Science Education.” Science and Education 9: 91–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, H. 2000b. “Kuhn's Account of Family Resemblance: A Solution to the Problem of Wide-Open Texture.” Erkenntnis 52: 313–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, H. 2001a. On Kuhn. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth
Andersen, H., 2001b. “Reference and Resemblance.” Philosophy of Science 69 (Proceedings)Google Scholar
Andersen, H., Barker, P., and Chen, X.. 1996. “Kuhn's Mature Philosophy of Science and Cognitive Psychology.” Philosophical Psychology 9: 347–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, S., Gleitman, L., and Gleitman, H.. 1983. “On What Some Concepts Might Not Be.” Cognition 13: 263–308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltas, A., K. Gavroglu, and V. Kindi. 2000. “A Discussion with Thomas S. Kuhn.” In Kuhn (2000), pp. 255–323. Based on a discussion in Athens, Greece, October 19–21, 1995
Barker, P. 1986. “Wittgenstein and the Authority of Science.” In: W. Leinfellner and F. Wuketits, eds. The Tasks of Contemporary Philosophy: Proceedings of the Xth International Wittgenstein Symposium. Vienna: Verlag Holdner-Pichler-Tempsky, pp. 265–7
Barker, P., 1993. “The Optical Theory of Comets from Apian to Kepler.” Physis 30: 1–25Google Scholar
Barker, P. 1996. “Understanding Change and Continuity.” In: F. Ragep and S. Ragep, eds. Tradition, Transmission, Transformation. Leiden: Brill, pp. 527–50
Barker, P.. 1999. “Copernicus and the Critics of Ptolemy.” Journal for the History of Astronomy 30: 343–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, P. 2001. “Incommensurability and Conceptual Change during the Copernican Revolution.” In: H. Sankey and P. Hoyningen-Huene, eds. Incommensurability and Related Matters. Boston Studies in the Philosophy of Science. Boston: Kluwer, pp. 241–73
Barker, P. Unpublished. “Kuhn, Incommensurability and Cognitive Science.” Paper presented at the conference “Kuhn Reconsidered,” Virginia Tech, Blacksburg, VA, March 2000
Barker, P., and Goldstein, B. R.. 1988. “The Role of Comets in the Copernican Revolution.” Studies in the History and Philosophy of Science 19: 299–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, P., and B. R. Goldstein. 2001. “Theological Foundations of Kepler's Astronomy.” In: J. H. Brooke, M. J. Osler, and J. van der Meer, eds. Osiris 16: 88–113CrossRef
Barsalou, L. W. 1982. “Ad Hoc Categories.” Memory and Cognition 11: 211–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. 1985. “Ideals, Central Tendency, and Frequency of Instantiation as Determinants of Graded Structure in Categories.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11: 629–54Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. 1987. “The Instability of Graded Structure: Implications for the Nature of Concepts.” In: U. Neisser, ed. Concepts and Conceptual Development: Ecological and Intellectual Factors in Categorization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 101–40
Barsalou, L. W. 1990. “On the Indistinguishability of Exemplar Memory and Abstraction in Category Representation.” In: T. Srull and R. Wyer, eds. Advances in Social Cognition, Vol. 3. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 61–88
Barsalou, L. W. 1991. “Deriving Categories to Achieve Goals.” In: G. H. Bower, ed. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, Vol. 27. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–64
Barsalou, L. W. 1992. “Frames, Concepts, and Conceptual Fields.” In: A. Lehrer and E. Kittay, eds. Frames, Fields and Contrasts: New Essays in Semantical and Lexical Organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 21–74
Barsalou, L. W., 1999. “Perceptual Symbol Systems.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 577–609Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W., and C. Hale. 1993. “Components of Conceptual Representation: From Feature-Lists to Recursive Frames.” In: I. Mechelen, J. Hampton, R. Michalski, and P. Theuns, eds. Categories and Concepts: Theoretical Views and Inductive Data Analysis. New York: Academic Press, pp. 97–144
Barsalou, L. W., and D. Sewell. 1984. Constructing Representations of Categories from Different Points of View. Emory Cognition Project Report No. 2. Atlanta: Emory University
Barsalou, L. W., K. Solomon, and L. Wu. 1999. “Perceptual Simulation in Conceptual Tasks.” In: M. Hiraga, C. Sinha, and S. Wilcox, eds. Cultural, Typological, and Psychological Perspectives in Cognitive Linguistics: The Proceedings of the 4th Conference of the International Cognitive Linguistics Association, Vol. 3. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 209–28
Bloor, D. 1976. Knowledge and Social Imagery. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul
Brewer, W. 2000. “Bartlett's Concept of the Schema and Its Impact on Theories of Knowledge Representation in Contemporary Cognitive Psychology.” In: A. Saito, ed. Bartlett, Culture and Cognition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, pp. 69–89
Cantor, N., Smith, E. E., French, R. D., and Mezzich, J.. 1980. “Psychiatric Diagnosis as Prototype Organization.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 89: 181–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X. 1990. “Local Incommensurability and Communicability.” In: A. Fine, M. Forbes, and L. Wessels, eds. PSA 1990, Vol. 1. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association, pp. 67–76
Chen, X. 1997. “Thomas Kuhn's Latest Notion of Incommensurability.” Journal for General Philosophy of Science 28: 257–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., 2001. “Perceptual Symbols and Taxonomy Comparison.” Philosophy of Science 69: 68 (special Proceedings issue): 200–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., Andersen, H., and Barker, P.. 1998. “Kuhn's Theory of Scientific Revolutions and Cognitive Psychology.” Philosophical Psychology 11: 5–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, X., and Barker, P.. 2000. “Continuity Through Revolutions: A Frame-Based Account of Conceptual Change During Scientific Revolutions.” Philosophy of Science 67 (Proceedings): S208–S223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conklin, H. C. 1969. “Lexicographical Treatment of Folk Taxonomies.” In: S. A. Tyler, ed. Cognitive Anthropology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 41–59
Curd, M., and J. A. Cover. 1998. Philosophy of Science: The Central Issues. New York: W. W. Norton
Ekman, P., W. V. Friesen, and P. Ellsworth. 1972. Emotion in the Human Face. Elmsford, NY: Pergamon
Erreich, A., and Valian, V.. 1979. “Children's Internal Organization of Locative Categories.” Child Development 50: 1070–77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, J. 1998. The History and Practice of Ancient Astronomy. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Gadow, H. 1892. “On the Classification of Birds.” Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London: 229–56
Hacking, I. 1993. “Working in a New World: The Taxonomic Solution.” In: P. Horwich, ed. World Changes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 275–310
Heider, E. R. 1972. “Universals in Color Naming and Memory.” Journal of Experimental Psychology 93: 10–20. (E. Heider later published under the name E. Rosch.)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homa, D. 1984. “On the Nature of Categories.” In: G. Bower, ed. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory, Vol. 18. New York: Academic Press, pp. 49–94
Homa, D., and Vosburgh, R.. 1976. “Category Breadth and the Abstraction of Prototypical Information.” Journal of Experimental Psychology – Human Learning and Memory 2: 322–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoyningen-Huene, P. 1993. Reconstructing Scientific Revolutions: Thomas S. Kuhn's Philosophy of Science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Hoyningen-Huene, P., Oberheim, E., and Andersen, H.. 1996. “On Incommensurability.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Science 27: 131–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. 1971. “Taxonomy and Semantic Contrast.” Language 47: 866–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. 1959. “The Essential Tension: Tradition and Innovation in Scientific Research.” In Kuhn (1977), pp. 225–39
Kuhn, T. S., 1961. “The Function of Measurement in Modern Physical Science.” Isis 52: 161–93; reprinted in Kuhn (1977), pp. 178–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. 1962. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kuhn, T. S. 1970a. “Postscript – 1969” in The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 2nd. ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 174–210
Kuhn, T. S. 1970b. “Logic of Discovery or Psychology of Research.” In: I. Lakatos and A. Musgrave, eds. Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–24; reprinted in Kuhn (1977), pp. 266–92
Kuhn, T. S. 1970c. “Reflections on My Critics.” In: I. Lakatos and A. Musgrave, eds. Criticism and the Growth of Knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 231–78
Kuhn, T. S. 1974. “Second Thoughts on Paradigms.” In: F. Suppe, ed. The Structure of Scientific Theories. Urbana: University of Illinois Press, pp. 459–82 plus discussion, pp. 500–17. Kuhn's essay is reprinted in Kuhn (1977), pp. 293–319
Kuhn, T. S., 1976. “Theory-Change as Structure-Change: Comments on the Sneed Formalism.” Erkenntnis 10: 179–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. 1977. The Essential Tension: Selected Studies in Scientific Tradition and Change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kuhn, T. S. 1979. “Metaphor in Science.” In: A. Ortony, ed. Metaphor in Science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 410–19
Kuhn, T. S. 1983a. “Commensurability, Comparability, and Communicability.” In: P. Asquith and T. Nickles, eds. PSA 1982, Vol. 2. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association, pp. 669–88
Kuhn, T. S. 1983b. “Response to Commentaries [on Commensurablity, Comparability, Communicability.]” In: P. D. Asquith and T. Nickles, eds. PSA 1982, Vol. 2. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association, pp. 712–16
Kuhn, T. S. 1989. “Possible Worlds in History of Science.” In: S. Allen, ed. Possible Worlds in Humanities, Arts and Sciences. New York: Walter de Gruyter, pp. 9–32
Kuhn, T. S. 1990. “Dubbing and Redubbing: The Vulnerability of Rigid Designation.” In: C. Savage, ed. Scientific Theory. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press, pp. 298–301
Kuhn, T. S. 1991. “The Road Since Structure.” In: A. Fine, M. Forbes, and L. Wessels, eds. PSA 1990, Vol. 2. East Lansing, MI: Philosophy of Science Association, pp. 3–13
Kuhn, T. S. 1993. “Afterwords.” In: P. Horwich, ed. World Changes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 311–41
Kuhn, T. S. 1996. The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 3rd ed. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Kuhn, T. S. 2000. The Road since Structure. J. Conant and J. Haugeland, eds. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Lakatos, I. 1977. The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Lattis, J. 1994. Between Copernicus and Galileo: Christopher Clavius and the Collapse of Ptolemaic Astronomy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Laudan, L., Donovan, A., Laudan, R., Barker, P., Brown, H., Leplin, J., Thagard, P., and Wykstra, S.. 1986. “Testing Theories of Scientific Change.” Synthese 69: 141–223CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mervis, C. B., and Pani, J. R.. 1980. “Acquisition of Basic Object Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 12: 496–522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Minsky, M. 1975. “A Framework for Representing Knowledge.” In: P. Winston, ed. The Psychology of Computer Vision. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 211–77
Nersessian, N. 1998. “Kuhn and the Cognitive Revolution.” Configurations 6: 87–120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newton, A. 1893. A Dictionary of Birds. London: Adam and Charles Black
Ray, J. 1678. The Ornithology of Francis Willughby. London: John Martyn
Rosch, E. 1973a. “Natural Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 4: 328–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. 1973b. “On the Internal Structure of Perceptual and Semantic Categories.” In: T. E. Moore, ed. Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language. New York: Academic Press, pp. 111–44
Rosch, E. 1987. “Wittgenstein and Categorization Research in Cognitive Psychology.” In: M. Chapman and R. A. Dixon, eds. Meaning and the Growth of Understanding: Wittgenstein's Significance for Developmental Psychology. Berlin: Springer, pp. 151–66
Rosch, E., and Mervis, C. B.. 1975. “Family Resemblances: Studies in the Internal Structures of Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 7: 573–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Gray, W., Johnson, D., and Boyes-Braem, P.. 1976. “Basic Objects in Natural Categories.” Cognitive Psychology 8: 382–439CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sankey, H. 1994. The Incommensurability Thesis. Aldershot: Avebury
Shapin, S. 1982. “History of Science and Its Sociological Reconstruction.” History of Science 20: 157–211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sibley, C., and J. Ahlquist. 1990. Phylogeny and Classification of Birds: A Study in Molecular Evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press
Smith, E., and D. Medin. 1981. Categories and Concepts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Sundevall, C. 1889. Sundevall's Tentamen. London: Porter
Tversky, B., and Hemenway, K.. 1984. “Objects, Parts, and Categories.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113: 169–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, T. M., Freyer, M. L., and Aiken, L. S.. 1977. “Development of Visual Pattern Classification in Preschool Children: Prototypes and Distinctive Features.” Developmental Psychology 13: 577–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1953. Philosophical Investigations. New York: Macmillan

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Kuhn on Concepts and Categorization
    • By Peter Barker, Professor of History of Science, The University of Oklahoma, Xiang Chen, Associate Professor, California Lutheran University, Hanne Andersen, Assistant Professor in the Department of Medical Philosophy and Clinical Theory, University of Copenhagen
  • Edited by Thomas Nickles, University of Nevada, Reno
  • Book: Thomas Kuhn
  • Online publication: 07 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511613975.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Kuhn on Concepts and Categorization
    • By Peter Barker, Professor of History of Science, The University of Oklahoma, Xiang Chen, Associate Professor, California Lutheran University, Hanne Andersen, Assistant Professor in the Department of Medical Philosophy and Clinical Theory, University of Copenhagen
  • Edited by Thomas Nickles, University of Nevada, Reno
  • Book: Thomas Kuhn
  • Online publication: 07 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511613975.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Kuhn on Concepts and Categorization
    • By Peter Barker, Professor of History of Science, The University of Oklahoma, Xiang Chen, Associate Professor, California Lutheran University, Hanne Andersen, Assistant Professor in the Department of Medical Philosophy and Clinical Theory, University of Copenhagen
  • Edited by Thomas Nickles, University of Nevada, Reno
  • Book: Thomas Kuhn
  • Online publication: 07 December 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511613975.010
Available formats
×