Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:59:29.987Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Embodiment

from Part I - Talk as Social Action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 June 2022

Amelia Church
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Amanda Bateman
Affiliation:
Swansea University
Get access

Summary

The role of embodiment in social interactions has attracted increasing attention in the last decade, both in the area of conversation analysis and that of cognitive science. Embodiment refers to all aspects of nonverbal, bodily behaviour, such as body posture and orientation, hand movements and gaze. This chapter will explain the concept of embodiment in both cognitive science and conversation-analytically informed research on social interaction, will present a state-of-the art review of research on embodiment in childhood interaction and will make clear the implications of this research for embodied practices in interactions with children, especially for childhood educators.

Type
Chapter
Information
Talking with Children
A Handbook of Interaction in Early Childhood Education
, pp. 78 - 96
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alibali, M., and Nathan, M. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateman, A., and Roberts, P. (2018). Morality at play: pretend play in five-year-old children. Research on Children and Social Interaction, 2(2), 195212.Google Scholar
Bateman, M. (2020). Young children’s affective stance through embodied displays of emotion during tellings. Text & Talk, 40(5), 653668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burdelski, M. (2012). Language socialization and politeness routines. In Duranti, A., Ochs, E., and Schiefferlin, B. (eds.), The Handbook of Language Socialization (pp. 275295). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Burdelski, M. (2020). Teacher compassionate touch in a Japanese preschool. Social interaction. Video-Based Studies of Human Sociality, 3(1). https://doi.org/10.7146/si.v3i1.120248Google Scholar
Call, J., and Carpenter, M. (2002).Three sources of information in social learning. In Dautenhahn, K. (ed.), Complex Adaptive Systems. Imitation in Animals and Artifacts (pp. 211228). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A. (2010). Shepherding the child: embodied directive sequences in parent–child interactions. Text & Talk, 30(1), 125.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A. (2015). The coordination of talk and touch in adults’ directives to children: touch and social control. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 48(2), 152175.Google Scholar
Cekaite, A. (2016). Touch as social control: haptic organization of attention in adult-child interactions. Journal of Pragmatics, 92, 3042.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S., and Ogston, W. D. (1971). Speech and body motion synchrony of speaker-hearer. In Horton, D. L. and Henkings, J. (eds.), Perception of Language (pp. 150173). Columbus, OH: Merill.Google Scholar
Condon, W. S., and Sander, L. W. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183, 99101.Google Scholar
Dalgren, S. (2017). Questions and answers, a seesaw and embodied action: how a preschool teacher and children accomplish educational practice. In Bateman, A. and Church, A. (eds.), Children’s Knowledge-in-Interaction. Studies in Conversation Analysis (pp. 37–56). Singapore: Springer.Google Scholar
Filipi, A. (2009). Toddler and Parent Interaction: The Organisation of Gaze, Pointing, and Vocalisation. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2009). Philosophical antecedents to situated cognition. In Robbins, P. and Aydede, M., (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Situated Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, R., and Mushin, I. (2017). Epistemic trajectories in the classroom: how children respond to informing sequences. In Bateman, A. and Church, A. (eds.), Children’s Knowledge-in-Interaction. Studies in Conversation Analysis (pp. 1336). Singapore: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerofsky, S. (2012). Mathematical learning and gesture: character viewpoint and observer viewpoint in students’ gestured graphs of functions. In Coletta, J.-M. and Guidetti, M. (eds.), Gesture and Multimodal Development (pp. 199220). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gogathe, L. J., and Bahrick, L. E. (2001). Intersensory redundancy and 7-month-old infants’ memory for arbitrary syllable-object relations. Infancy, 2(2), 219231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W., and Mitchell, Z. A. (2009). Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science, 20(3), 267272.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (1994). Professional vision. American Anthropologist, 96(3), 606633.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment within situated human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(10), 14891522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2013). The co-operative, transformative organization of human action and knowledge. Journal of Pragmatics, 46(1), 823.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (2018). Co-operative action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (2007). Participation and embodiment in preadolescent girls’ assessment activity. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 40(4), 353375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (2017). Haptic sociality: the embodied interactive constitution of intimacy through touch. In Meyer, C., Streeck, J., and Jordan, S. (eds.), Intercorporeality. Emerging Socialities in Interaction (pp. 73102). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., and Cekaite, A. (2013). Calibration in directive/response sequences in family interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 46, 122138.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., and Cekaite, A. (2018). Embodied Family Interaction. Practices of Control, Care, and Mundane Activities. London, New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Heritage, J. (1984). A change-of-state-token and aspects of its sequential placement. In Atkinson, J. and Heritage, J. (eds.), Structures of Social Action. Studies in Conversation Analysis (pp. 219–248). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hindmarsh, J., Reynolds, P., and Dunne, S. (2011). Exhibiting understanding: the body in apprenticeship. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 289503.Google Scholar
Hostetter, A., and Alibali, M. (2008). Visible embodiment: gestures as simulated actions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(3), 495514.Google Scholar
Jakonen, T. (2015). Handling knowledge: using classroom materials to construct and interpret information requests. Journal of Pragmatics, 89, 100112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakonen, T., and Niemi, K. (2020). Managing participation and turn-taking in children’s digital activities: touch in blocking a peer’s hand. Social Interaction. Video-Based Studies of Human Sociality, 3(1). https://doi.org/10.7146/si.v3i1.120250Google Scholar
Kääntä, L., and Piirainen-Marsh, A. (2013). Manual guiding in peer group interaction: a resource for organizing a practical classroom task. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 46(4), 322343.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2010). Spacing and orientation in co-present interaction. In Esposito, A., Campbell, N., Vogel, C., Hussain, A., and Nijholt, A. (eds.), Development of Multimodal Interfaces: Active Listening and Synchrony. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol. 5967. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/978–3-642–12397-9_1Google Scholar
Kern, F. (2018a). Clapping hands with the teacher. What synchronization reveals about learning. Journal of Pragmatics, 125, 2842.Google Scholar
Kern, F. (2018b). Mastering the body. Correcting bodily conduct in adult-child interaction. Research on Children and Social Interaction, 2(2), 213234.Google Scholar
Kern, F. (2020). Interactional and multimodal resources in children’s game explanations. Research on Children and Social Interaction, 4(1), 727.Google Scholar
Kidwell, M. (2011). Epistemics and embodiment in the interaction with very young children. In Stivers, T., Mondada, L., and Steensig, J. (eds.), The Morality of Knowledge in Conversation (pp. 257283). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1981). Metaphors We Live By. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (2007). ‘Like me’: a foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of Perception. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mondada, L. (2011). Understanding as an embodied, situated and sequential achievement in interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(2), 542552.Google Scholar
Mondada, L. (2019). Challenges of multimodality: language and the body in social interaction. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 20(3), 336366.Google Scholar
Mundy, P., Block, J., Delgado, C., Pomares, Y., Hecke, A.V., and Parlade, M. A. (2007). Individual differences and the development of joint attention. Child Development, 78(3), 938954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nomikou, I., and Rohlfing, K. (2011). Language does something: body action and language in maternal input to three-month-olds. IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, 3(2), 113128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nomikou, I., Koke, M., and Rohlfing, K. (2017). Verbs in mothers’ input to six-month-olds: synchrony between presentation, meaning and actions is related to later verb acquisition. Brain Sciences, 7(5), 52. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci7050052CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rambusch, J., and Ziemke, T. (2005). The role of embodiment in situated learning. In Bara, B. G., Barsalou, L., and Bucciarelli, M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 11131118). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Roth, W.-M. (2002). Gestures: their role in teaching and learning. Review of Educational Research, 71, 365392.Google Scholar
Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft. The manufacture of meaning. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., and LeBaron, C. (2011). Embodied interaction in the material world: an introduction. In Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., and LeBaron, C. (eds.), Embodied Interaction. Language and Body in the Material World (pp. 126). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Thorne, S. L., Hellermann, J., Jones, A., and Lester, D. (2015). Interactional practices and artifact orientation in mobile augmented reality game play. Psychology Journal, 13(2–3), 259286.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., and Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121125.Google Scholar
Trevarthan, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In Braten, S. (ed.), Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny (pp. 1546). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tulbert, E., and Goodwin, M. H. (2011). Choreographies of attention. Multimodality in a routine activity. In Streeck, J., Goodwin, C., and LeBaron, C. (eds.), Embodied Interaction. Language and Body in the Material World (pp. 7992). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. A., and Foglia, L. (2017). Embodied cognition. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Website. Last modified 8 December 2015. Accessed 1 October 2020. Available at: plato.stanford.edu/entries/embodied-cognitionGoogle Scholar
Wilson., A., and Golonka, S. (2013). Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Frontiers in Psychology (online). https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058Google Scholar
Zemel, A., and Koschmann, T. (2014). ‘Put your finger right in here’: learnability and instructed experience. Discourse Studies, 6(2), 163183.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×