Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T07:06:52.375Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Determining the Presence of Habitual Travel Route Networks in Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus morio) in Kutai National Park, Borneo

from Part II - GIS Analysis in Fine-Scale Space

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 January 2021

Francine L. Dolins
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Dearborn
Christopher A. Shaffer
Affiliation:
Grand Valley State University, Michigan
Leila M. Porter
Affiliation:
Northern Illinois University
Jena R. Hickey
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Nathan P. Nibbelink
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Recent field research has shown that some wild primates possess detailed knowledge of the location and quality of many resources in their home ranges (Janson & Byrne 2007). Understanding how nonhuman primates (hereafter primates) store, organize, and use such information is important for understanding the evolution of human spatial cognition. Knowledge of primate spatial cognition can also inform conservation initiatives for threatened populations by improving protected habitat such that primates are encouraged to travel through desired areas and discouraged from traveling near human development, thereby reducing primate–human conflict.

Type
Chapter
Information
Spatial Analysis in Field Primatology
Applying GIS at Varying Scales
, pp. 204 - 224
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Asensio, N., Brockelman, W. Y., Malaivijitnond, S., and Reichard, U. H. 2011. Gibbon travel paths are goal oriented. Animal Cognition 14(3): 395405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, L. A., and Byrne, R. W. 2009. Sex differences in the movement patterns of free-ranging chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii): foraging and border checking. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 64(2): 247255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruggeman, J. E., Garrott, R. A., White, P. J., Watson, F. G. R., and Wallen, R. 2007. Covariates affecting spatial variability in bison travel behavior in Yellowstone National Park. Ecological Applications 17(5): 14111423.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Noser, R., Bates, L. A., and Jupp, P. E. 2009. How did they get here from there? Detecting changes of direction in terrestrial ranging. Animal Behaviour 77(3): 619631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruse, H., and Wehner, R. 2011. No need for a cognitive map: decentralized memory for insect navigation. PLoS Computational Biology 7(3): e1002009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Department of Natural Resources WA. 2004. Standards and Guidelines for Land Surveying Using Global Positioning System Methods. State Department of Natural Resources, Olympia, WAGoogle Scholar
Di Fiore, A., and Suarez, S. A. 2007. Route-based travel and shared routes in sympatric spider and woolly monkeys: cognitive and evolutionary implications. Animal Cognition 10(3): 317329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolins, F. L. 2009. Captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus oedipus) use landmarks to localize hidden food items. American Journal of Primatology 71 : 316323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolins, F. L., Klimowicz, C., Kelley, J., and Menzel, C. R. 2014. Using virtual reality to investigate comparative spatial cognitive abilities in chimpanzees and humans. American Journal of Primatology 76 : 496513.Google Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. 1988. Orangutan diet, range, and activity at Tanjung Puting, Central Borneo. International Journal of Primatology 9(1): 135.Google Scholar
Galdikas, B. M. F. and Vasey, P. 1992. Why are orangutans so smart? Ecological and social hypotheses. Pages 183224 in Social Processes and Mental Abilities in Non-Human Primates. Burton, F. D. (Ed.). The Edwin Mellen Press, Lewiston, NY.Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. and Cramer, A. E. 1996. Computations on metric maps in mammals: getting oriented and choosing a multi-destination route. Journal of Experimental Biology 199(1): 211217.Google Scholar
Hopkins, M. E. 2010. Mantled howler (Alouatta palliata) arboreal pathway networks: relative impacts of resource availability and forest structure. International Journal of Primatology 32(1): 238258.Google Scholar
Janmaat, K. R. L., Byrne, R. W., and Zuberbühler, K. 2006. Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Animal Behaviour 72(4): 797807.Google Scholar
Janson, C. H. and Byrne, R. 2007. What wild primates know about resources: opening up the black box. Animal Cognition 10(3): 357367.Google Scholar
Knott, C., Beaudrot, L., Snaith, T., et al. 2008. Female–female competition in Bornean orangutans. International Journal of Primatology 29(4): 975997.Google Scholar
Leighton, M. 1993. Modeling dietary selectivity by Bornean orangutans: evidence for integration of multiple criteria in fruit selection. International Journal of Primatology 14(2): 257313.Google Scholar
Lührs, M.-L., Dammhahn, M., Kappeler, P. M., and Fichtel, C. 2009. Spatial memory in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Animal Cognition 12(4): 599609.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacKinnon, J. 1974. The behaviour and ecology of wild orangutans. Animal Behaviour 22: 374.Google Scholar
Milton, K. 1981. Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to primate mental development. American Anthropologist 83: 534548.Google Scholar
Ministry of Environment BC. 2001. British Columbia Standards, Specifications and Guidelines for Resource Surveys Using Global Positioning System (GPS) Technology. Ministry of Environment, Lands and Parks, Victoria, BC.Google Scholar
Morrogh-Bernard, H., Husson, S. J., Knott, C. D., et al. 2009. Orangutan activity budgets and diet. Pages 119134 in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioural Ecology and Conservation. Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., and van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.). Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Normand, E. and Boesch, C. 2009. Sophisticated Euclidean maps in forest chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour 77(5): 11951201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noser, R. and Byrne, R. W. 2010. How do wild baboons (Papio ursinus) plan their routes? Travel among multiple high-quality food sources with inter-group competition. Animal Cognition 13(1): 145155.Google Scholar
Porter, L. M. and Garber, P. A. 2012. Foraging and spatial memory in wild Weddell’s saddleback tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis weddelli) when moving between distant and out-of-sight goals. International Journal of Primatology 34(1): 3048.Google Scholar
Poucet, B. 1993. Spatial cognitive maps in animals: new hypotheses on their structure and neural mechanisms. Psychological Review 100(2): 163182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B. L. and Evans, S. 2001. Episodic memory in primates. American Journal of Primatology 55(2): 7185.Google Scholar
Setiawan, A., Nugroho, T. S., and Pudyatmoko, S. 2009. A survey of Miller’s grizzled surili, Presbytis hosei canicrus, in East Kalimantan, Indonesia. Primate Conservation 24(1): 139143.Google Scholar
Singleton, I., Knott, C. D., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Wich, S. A., and van Schaik, C. P. 2009. Ranging behavior of orangutan females and social organization. Pages 205214 in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioural Ecology and Conservation. Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., and van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.). Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Squires, J. R., DeCesare, N. J., Olson, L. E., et al. 2013. Combining resource selection and movement behavior to predict corridors for Canada lynx at their southern range periphery. Biological Conservation 157: 187195.Google Scholar
Thorpe, S. K. and Crompton, R. H. 2009. Orangutan positional behaviour. Pages 3347 in Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behavioural Ecology and Conservation. Wich, S. A., Utami Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., and van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.). Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×