Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T05:05:56.546Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Love, Diminutives, and Gender Socialization in Andean Mother–Child Narrative Conversations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Allyssa McCabe
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Lowell
Alison L. Bailey
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Gigliana Melzi
Affiliation:
New York University
Get access

Summary

Key Words: diminutive, evaluative morphology, emotion, narrative, evaluation, Spanish, Andes, maternal speech

ABSTRACT

This chapter investigates how emotional words and diminutives function as evaluative resources within mother–child narrative conversations. Participants included 32 Indigenous Spanish-speaking mother–child pairs from the southern Ecuadorian Andes. Mothers were asked to record interactions in which they participated in narrative conversations with their child. Findings suggest that diminutives played a salient part in the socialization of emotion in this Indigenous community. Both quantitative and qualitative analyses indicated gender differences in uses of these types of evaluation and, in particular, in how diminutives and emotional words were used together, with 5-year-old girls hearing significantly more diminutives in emotional utterances than 3-year-old girls and more than boys of both age groups. Implications for narrative evaluation and language socialization are discussed.

INTRODUCTION

Narratives are often defined as stories about actual or imaginary past events (McCabe, 1991). Early and foundational work on narrative (e.g., Labov & Waletzky, 1967) identified evaluation as a central narrative component. As Labov and Waletzky (1967) demonstrated, a narrative's referential functions might be carried out perfectly well; however, without evaluation, the narrative tends to be difficult to understand and lacks significance – in their words, “it has no point” (p. 33). Daiute and Nelson (1997) extended this work, pointing out that as children develop narrative discourse skills, evaluation helps them learn how to situate or position themselves within society.

Type
Chapter
Information
Spanish-Language Narration and Literacy
Culture, Cognition, and Emotion
, pp. 119 - 142
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, S., Kuebli, J., Boyle, P. A., & Fivush, R. (1995). Gender differences in parent–child conversations about past emotions: A longitudinal investigation. Sex Roles, 33, 309–323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alonso, A. (1961). La base lingüística del español Americano. Estudios lingüísticos. Temas hispanoamericanos. Madrid: Gredos.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. (1997). Evaluative morphology: In search of universals. Studies in Language, 21, 533–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belote, J., & Belote, L. (1997). The Saraguros, 1962–1997: A very brief overview. Retrieved June 21, 2007, from http://www.saraguro.org/overview.htm.
Berko Gleason, J., Perlmann, R. U., Ely, R., & Evans, D. W. (1994). The babytalk register: Parents' use of diminutives. In Sokolov, J. L. & Snow, C. A. (Eds.), Handbook of research in language development using CHILDES. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R. (1997). Narrative theory and narrative development: The Labovian impact. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7(1–4), 235–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cervantes, C. A., & Callanan, M. A. (1998). Labels and explanations in mother–child emotion talk: Age and gender differentiation. Developmental Psychology, 34, 88–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daiute, C., & Nelson, K. (1997). Making sense of the sense-making function of narrative evaluation. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 7, 207–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U., & Barbaresi, L. M. (1994). Morphopragmatics: Diminutives and intensifiers in Italian, German, and other languages. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 448–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, C. A. (1977). Babytalk as a simplified register. In Snow, C. E. and Ferguson, C. A. (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 209–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fivush, R. (1991). Gender and emotion in mother–child conversations about the past. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 1, 325–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., Brotman, M. A., Buckner, J. P., & Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent–child emotion narratives. Sex Roles, 42, 233–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fivush, R., & Buckner, J. P. (2000). Gender, sadness, and depression: The development of emotional focus through gendered discourse. In Fischer, A. H. (Ed.), Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives (pp. 232–253). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannagan, D., & Perese, S. (1998). Emotional references in mother–daughter and mother–son dyads' conversations about school. Sex Roles, 39(5–6), 353–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, P., & Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socialization: Reproduction and continuity, transformation and change. Annual Review of Anthropology, 31, 339–361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Inchaurralde, C. (1997). Space, reference, and emotional involvement. In Niemeier, S. & Dirven, R. (Eds.), The language of emotions: Conceptualization, expression, and theoretical foundation (pp.135–154). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Invernizzi, M. A., & Abouzeid, M. P. (1995). One story map does not fit all: A cross-cultural analysis of children's written story retellings. Journal of Narrative and Life History, 5, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, D. (1996). Universal tendencies in the semantics of the diminutive. Language, 72, 533–578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kempe, V., & Brooks, P. J. (1998). Do diminutives facilitate the learning of Russian gender? In Gernsbacher, M. & Derry, S. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (1231). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kempe, V., Brooks, P. J., Mironova, N., & Fedorova, O. (2003). Diminutivisation supports gender acquisition in Russian children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 471–485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, K. A. (2001).Language revitalization processes and prospects: Quichua in the Ecuadorian Andes. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
King, K., & Haboud, M. (2002). Language planning and policy in Ecuador. Current Issues in Language Planning, 3, 359–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, K. A., & Hornberger, N. H. (2004). Introduction. Why a special issue on Quechua? International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 167, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, K. A., & Melzi, G. (2004). Intimacy, imitation and language learning: Spanish diminutives in mother–child conversations.First Language, 24, 241–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, W., & Waletzky, J. (1967). Narrative analysis: Oral versions of personal experience. In Helm, J. (Ed.), Essays on the verbal and visual arts (pp. 12–44). Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McCabe, A. (1991). Preface: Structure as a way of understanding. In McCabe, A. & Peterson, C. (Eds.), Developing narrative structure (pp. ix–xvii). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Melzi, G. (2000). Cultural variations in the construction of personal narratives: Central American and European American mothers' elicitation styles. Discourse Processes, 30, 153–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melzi, G., & Fernández, C. (2004). Talking about past emotions: Conversations between Peruvian mothers and their preschool children. Sex Roles, 50, 641–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melzi, G., & King, K. A. (2003). Spanish diminutives in mother–child conversations. Journal of Child Language, 30, 280–304.Google ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E. (1986). Introduction. In Schieffelin, B. B. & Ochs, E. (Eds.), Language socialization across cultures (pp. 1–13). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ochs, E., & Capps, L. (2001). Living narrative: Creating lives in everyday storytelling. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Peterson, C., and McCabe, A. (1983). Developmental psycholinguistics: Three ways of looking at a child's narrative. New York: Plenum Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polanyi, L. (1981). What stories can tell us about their teller's world. Poetics Today, 2, 97–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scollon, R., & Scollon, S. (1981). Narrative, literacy and face in interethnic communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (2003). Cross-cultural pragmatics: The semantics of human interaction. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
,World Bank (2007). Ecuador poverty assessment. Washington, DC: The World Bank Group.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×