Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T04:23:41.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Human hunting seasonality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 August 2009

Rebecca Bliege Bird
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropological Sciences Stanford University Stanford CA 94305–2117 USA
Douglas W. Bird
Affiliation:
Department of Anthropological Sciences Stanford University Stanford CA 94305–2117 USA
Diane K. Brockman
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Charlotte
Carel P. van Schaik
Affiliation:
Universität Zürich
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Human hunting strategies, like those of many non-human primates, vary seasonally with fluctuations in prey abundance, encounter rates, and profitability (Winterhalder 1981; Smith 1991). Temporality in resource supply has profound social effects as well, and some of the earliest studies of hunter–gatherers emphasized the impact of seasonality on settlement size, mobility, general economic organization, and even property rights, religion, family structure, and the sexual division of labor (Mauss & Beuchat 1906; Thomson 1936). For Mauss and Beuchat (1906), seasonality meant temperature: they suggested that Inuit families were organized very differently in the summer than in the winter as a result of the nature of changes in foraging opportunities. For Thomson (1936), seasonality meant rainfall, commenting that the effect of distinct wet and dry seasons in northern Australia might lead one to think that they were observing two different “tribes” of people. Anthropological interest in seasonality and its effects on human social organization has waned since then, frustrated by an inability to find correlations between seasonality and human behavior. Our goal in this chapter is to explore the utility of two approaches to understanding the relationship between seasonality and social behavior. One attempts to use comparative ecological data across groups to explain differences in aspects of social and economic behavior such as mobility and land tenure decisions; the other examines how different individuals within a group may respond differently to resource seasonality.

Type
Chapter
Information
Seasonality in Primates
Studies of Living and Extinct Human and Non-Human Primates
, pp. 243 - 266
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alberts, S. (1996). Mate guarding constrains foraging activity of male baboons. Animal behavior, 51, 1269–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altman, J. (1987). Hunter Gatherers Today: An Aboriginal Economy in North Australia. Canberra: AIATSIS.Google Scholar
Altmann, S. (1998). Foraging for Survival. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bahuchet, S. (1988). Food supply uncertainty among the Aka Pygmies (Lbaye, Central African Republic). In Coping with Uncertainty in Food Supply, ed. Gariene, I. and Harrison, G. A.. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 118–49.Google Scholar
Bailey, R. & Aunger, R. J. (1989). Net hunters vs. archers: variation in women's subsistence strategies in the Ituri forest. Human Ecology, 17, 273–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binford, L. R. (1980). Willow smoke and dog's tails: hunter–gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site formation. American Antiquity, 45, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binford, L. R.(2001). Constructing Frames of Reference: An Analytical Method for Archaeological Theory Building Using Hunter–Gatherer and Environmental Data Sets. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Bird, D. W. & Bliege Bird, R. (2005). Mardu children's hunting strategies in the Western Desert, Australia: implications for the evolution of human life histories. In Hunter–Gatherer Childhoods, ed. Hewlett, B. and Lamb, M.. New York: Aldine de Gruyter, in press.Google Scholar
Bliege Bird, R., Bird, D. W., & Smith, E. A. (2001). The hunting handicap: costly signaling in human male foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 50, 9–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boesch, C. & Boesch, H. (1984). Possible causes of sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 13, 415–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boinski, S. (1988). Sex differences in the foraging behavior of squirrel monkeys in a seasonal habitat. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 23, 177–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brand Miller, J., James, K., & Maggiore, P. (1993). Tables of Composition of Australian Aboriginal Foods. Canberra: Aboriginal Studies Press.Google Scholar
Cane, S. (1987). Australian aboriginal subsistence in the Western Desert. Human Ecology, 15, 391–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E. (1985). Coping with risk: reciprocity among the Basarwa of Northern Botswana. Man, 20, 454–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cashdan, E.(1992). Spatial organization and habitat use. In Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior, ed. Smith, E. A. and Winterhalder, B.. New York: Aldine de Gruyter, pp. 237–66.Google Scholar
Charnov, E. (1976). Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 129–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144, 517–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N. (1989). Hardworking Hadza grandmothers. In Comparative Socioecology: The Behavioral Ecology of Humans and Other Mammals, ed. Standen, V. & Foley, R. A.. London: Blackwell, pp. 341–66.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N.(1995). Hadza children's foraging: juvenile dependency, social arrangements and mobility among hunter-gatherers. Current Anthropology, 36, 688–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N.(2001a). Hunting and nuclear families: some lessons from the Hadza about men's work. Current Anthropology, 42, 681–709.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., O'Connell, J. F., & Blurton Jones, N.(2001b). Hadza meat sharing. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 113–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurtado, A. M. & Hill, K. (1990). Seasonality in a foraging society: variation in diet, work effort, fertility, and sexual division of labor among the Hiwi of Venezuela. Journal of Anthropological Research, 46, 293–346.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, R. A. (1997). The evolution of animal signals. In Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B.. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 155–78.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (1995). The Foraging Spectrum: Diversity in Hunter–Gatherer Lifeways. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Books.Google Scholar
Mather, J. R. (1962). Average Climatic Water Balance Data of the Continents. Part II: Asia (Excluding USSR). Centerton, NJ: Thornthwaite Associates Laboratory of Climatology.Google Scholar
Mauss, M. & Beuchat, H, (1906). Seasonal Variations of the Eskimo: A Study in Social Morphology. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1992). Chimpanzee Material Culture: Implications for Human Evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitani, J. & Watts, D. (2001). Why do chimpanzees hunt and share meat?Animal Behaviour, 61, 915–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nishida, T., Hasegawa, T., Hayaki, H., Takahata, Y., & Uehara, S. (1992). Meat sharing as a coalition strategy by an alpha male chimpanzee. In Topics in Primatology, Vol. 1, ed. Nishida, T., McGrew, W. C., Marler, P., Pickford, M. & Waal, F. B. M.. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press, pp. 159–74.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. (1991). Inujjuamiut Foraging Strategies: Evolutionary Ecology of an Arctic Hunting Economy. New York: Aldine.Google Scholar
Smith, E. A., Bliege Bird, R., & Bird, D. W. (2003). The benefits of costly signaling: Meriam turtle hunters and spearfishers. Behavioral Ecology, 14, 116–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, B. & Gillen, F. J. (1927). The Arunta: A Study of a Stone Age People. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stanford, C. (1996). The hunting ecology of wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolutionary ecology of Pliocene hominids. American Anthropologist, 98, 96–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanford, C., Wallis, J., Mpongo, E., & Goodall, J. (1994). Hunting decisions in wild chimpanzees. Behavior, 131, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephens, D. & Krebs, J. R. (1986). Foraging Theory. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, W. (1964). Tethered nomadism and water territoriality: an hypothesis. Proceedings of the 35th International Congress of Americanists, 2, 197–203.Google Scholar
Terborgh, J. (1983). Five New World Primates: A Study in Comparative Ecology. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, D. H. (1983). On Steward's models of Shoshonean sociopolitical organization: a great bias in the Great Basin? In The Development of Political Organization in North America, ed. Tooker, E.. Philadelphia: American Ethnological Society, pp. 59–68.Google Scholar
Thomson, D. (1936). Donald Thomson in Arnhem Land. Sydney: Gordon and Gotch.Google Scholar
Tonkinson, R. (1974). The Jigalong Mob: Aboriginal Victors of the Desert Crusade. Menlo Park, CA: Cummings.Google Scholar
Tonkinson, R.(1991). The Mardu Aborigines: Living the Dream in Australia's Desert. New York: Holt, Rhinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Tutin, C. (1979). Mating patterns and reproductive strategies in a community of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 6, 29–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, F. (1990). An ecological study of traditional aboriginal use of “country”: Martu in the Great and Little Sandy Deserts, Western Australia. Proceedings of the Ecological Society of Australia, 16, 23–37.Google Scholar
Winterhalder, B. (1981). Foraging strategies in the boreal forest: an analysis of Cree hunting and gathering. In Hunter Gatherer Foraging Strategies, ed. Winterhalder, B. & Smith, E. A.. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 66–98.Google Scholar
Winterhalder, B. & Smith, E. A. (2000). Analyzing adaptive strategies: human behavioral ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 51–72.3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: a selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×