Published online by Cambridge University Press: 23 January 2010
The year 1963 was a landmark for television in the United States. In that year, the major television networks instituted their half-hour news programs, and a majority of people for the first time reported in a Roper poll that their chief source of news was television (Barnouw 1982). Ever since, television has reigned supreme as the people's source of news in the US. Indeed, informants for the present study are living testaments to this assertion: while about 43 percent reported never reading Los Angeles' major daily newspaper over the course of the events, only 3 percent of the sample reported not watching television news coverage of the events the first night (see table A.2, appendix A). This latter finding echoes viewership figures for the city as whole. Following initial reports of the verdicts, viewer levels were not much above normal across the stations in the city. But by primetime hours (i.e., by 8 pm), news of the police acquittals had spread throughout the city, as had reports of “violence,” “looting,” and other events of “protest.” By this time, 77 percent of the households in the city were tuned to local news coverage of the events (Los Angeles Times, May 1, 1992).
If, as I advocated in the previous chapter, we conceptualize the television experience as a continuous meaning-making process shaped by texts, viewers and their intertextual memories, then the present chapter focuses on the first of these factors. From what position or positions did the news media speak? What encoded meanings did study informants have to work with or struggle against as they made sense of the selected news text?
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.