Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T01:22:02.740Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Pretend and Socio-dramatic Play in Evolutionary and Developmental Perspective

from Part I - Play and Ritual: Forms, Foundations and Evolution in Animals and Humans

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2017

Colin Renfrew
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Iain Morley
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Michael Boyd
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. 1989. Evolution of the human psyche, in The Human Revolution, eds. Mellars, P. & Stringer, C.. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, 455513.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. & Allen, C. 1998. Intentional communication and social play: how and why animals negotiate and agree to play, in Animal Play: Evolutionary, Comparative, and Ecological Perspectives, eds. Bekoff, M. & Byers, J. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 97114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blurton Jones, N. G. 1993. The lives of hunter-gatherer children: Effects of parental behavior and parental reproductive strategy, in Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior, eds. M. E. Pereira and L. A. Fairbanks. New York: Oxford University Press, 309–25.Google Scholar
Breuggeman, J. A. 1978. The function of adult play in free-ranging Macaca mulatto, in Social Play in Primates, ed. Smith, E. O.. New York: Academic Press, 169–91.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. 1986. Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. 2002. Human creativity: its cognitive basis, its evolution, and its connection with childhood pretence. British Journal of the Philosophy of Science, 53, 225–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christie, J. F. & Roskos, K. A. 2006. Standards, science, and the role of play in early literacy education, in Play=Learning, eds. Singer, D., Golinkoff, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K.. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 5773.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. 1989. Peacemaking Among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, S. 2005. The narrative worlds of what is and what if. Cognitive Development, 20, 514–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fein, G. G. 1975. A transformational analysis of pretending. Developmental Psychology, 11, 291–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fenson, L., Kagan, J., Kearsley, R. B. & Zelazo, P. 1972. The developmental progression of manipulative play in the first two years. Child Development, 47, 232–5.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S. 1999. Children’s lives in a Mayan village: a case of culturally constructed roles and activities, in Children’s Engagement in the World: Sociocultural Perspectives, ed. Göncü, A.. New York: Cambridge University Press, 2561.Google Scholar
Gaskins, S., Haight, W. & Lancy, D. F. 2007. The cultural construction of play, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 179202.Google Scholar
Gómez, J. C. & Martín-Andrade, B. 2005. Fantasy play in apes, in The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans, eds. Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K.. New York: Guilford, 139–72.Google Scholar
Göncü, , Patt, A., , M. B. & Kouba, E. 2002. Understanding young children’s pretend play in context, in Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development, eds. Smith, P. K. & Hart, C. H.. Oxford: Blackwell, 418–37.Google Scholar
Göncü, A. & Perone, A. 2005. Pretend play as a life-span activity. Topoi, 24, 137–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gosso, Y., Otta, E., Morais, M. L. S., Ribeiro, F. J. L. & Bussab, V. S. R. 2005. Play in hunter-gatherer society, in The Nature of Play: Great Apes and Humans, eds. Pellegrini, A. D. & Smith, P. K.. New York: Guilford, 213–53.Google Scholar
Gottman, J. M. 1983. How children become friends. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 48 no 3; serial no 201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haight, W. L. & Miller, P. J. 1993. Pretending at Home: Early Development in a Sociocultural Context. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. 1994. Understanding pretense, in Children’s Early Understanding of Mind, eds. Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P.. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum, 235–9.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. 2000. The Work of the Imagination. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. 2007. Hard work for the imagination, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 205–25.Google Scholar
Hayes, C. H. 1951. The Ape in our House. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Holland, P. 2003. We Don’t Play with Guns here. Philadelphia. PA: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Howes, C. & Matheson, C. C. 1992. Sequences in the development of competent play with peers. Social and social pretend play. Developmental Psychology, 28, 961–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarrold, C. 2003. A review of research into pretend play in autism. Autism, 7, 379–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarrold, C., Carruthers, P., Smith, P. K. & Boucher, J. 1994. Pretend play: is it metarepresentational? Mind and Language, 9, 445–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanlenberg, S. M. & Wrangham, R. W. 2010. Sex differences in chimpanzees' use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20, R1067–8.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. 2006. An Exploration of the Role of Executive Functions in the Symbolic Play of Children with High-Functioning Autism, Children with Asperger’s Disorder, and Typically Developing Children. Unpublished doctoral thesis, LaTrobe University, Australia.Google Scholar
Konner, M. 1972. Aspects of the developmental ethology of a forging people, in Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour, ed. Jones, N. Blurton. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 285304.Google Scholar
Lancy, D. F. 1996. Playing on the Mother Ground: Cultural Routines for Children’s Development. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S. 1994. Making sense of pretence, in Children’s Early Understanding of Mind, eds. Lewis, C. & Mitchell, P.. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum, 211–34.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, R. A., Smith, E. D. & Palmquist, C. M. 2013. The impact of pretend play on children’s development: a review of the evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacWhinney, B. 2005. Language evolution and human development, in Origins of the Social Mind, eds. Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F.. New York & London: Guilford Press, 383410.Google Scholar
Martini, M. 1994. Peer interactions in Polynesia: a view from the Marquesas. In Children’s Play in Diverse Cultures, eds. Roopnarine, J. L., Johnson, J. E. & Hooper, F. H.. Albany: State University of New York Press, 73103.Google Scholar
Matevia, M. L., Patterson, F. & Hillix, W. A. 2002. Pretend play in a signing gorilla, in Pretending and Imagination in Animals and Children, ed. Mitchell, R. W.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 285304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, R. 2007. Pretense in animals: the continuing relevance of children’s pretense, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 5175.Google Scholar
Morris, D. 1962. The Biology of Art. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Nicolopoulou, A. 2006. The interplay of play and narrative in children’s development: theoretical reflections and concrete examples, in Play and Development: Evolutionary, Sociocultural and Functional Perspectives, eds. Göncü, A. & Gaskins, S.. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 247–73.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F. & Jackson, J. P. 1973. The representation of imagines objects in action sequences: a developmental study. Child Development, 44, 309–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peller, L. E. 1954. Libidinal phases, ego development and play. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 9, 178–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T. & Leekam, S. R. 1994. Theory of mind is contagious: you catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65, 1228–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perone, A. & Göncü, A. 2014. Life-span pretend play in two communities. Mind, Culture and Activity, 21(3), 200–20.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. 1951. Play, Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Renfrew, C. 2007. Prehistory: The Making of the Human Mind. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. 1986. Ape Language: From Conditioned Response to Symbol. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slaughter, D. & Dombrowski, J. 1989. Cultural continuities and discontinuities: impact on social and pretend play, in The Ecological Context of Children’s Play, eds. Bloch, M. N. & Pellegrini, A. D.. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, 282310.Google Scholar
Smilansky, S. 1968. The Effects of Sociodramatic Play on Disadvantaged Preschool Children. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. 1988. Children’s play and its role in early development: a re-evaluation of the ‘play ethos’, in Psychological Bases for Early Education, ed. Pellegrini, A. D.. Chichester & New York: Wiley, 207–26.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. 2010. Children and Play. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Takhvar, M. & Smith, P. K. 1990. A review and critique of Smilansky’s classification scheme and the ‘nested hierarchy’ of play categories. Journal of Research in Early Childhood, 4, 112–22.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. 1999. Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, M., Cartwright, B. S. & Carlson, S. M. 1993. A developmental investigation of children’s imaginary companions. Developmental Psychology, 29, 276–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woolley, J. D. & Wellman, H. H. 1990. Young children’s understanding of realities, nonrealities, and appearances. Child Development, 64, 117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×