Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T20:39:10.486Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - From Play and Ritualisation to Ritual and Its Arts: Sources of Upper Pleistocene Ritual Practices in Lower Middle Pleistocene Ritualised and Play Behaviours in Ancestral Hominins

from Part I - Play and Ritual: Forms, Foundations and Evolution in Animals and Humans

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 December 2017

Colin Renfrew
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Iain Morley
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Michael Boyd
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alland, A., Jr. 1983. Playing With Form. New York: Columbia University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartels, A. & Zeki, S., 2004. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. Neuroimage 21(3), 1155–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F. & Leary, M. R., 1995. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin 117(3), 497529.Google ScholarPubMed
Beck, J. & Forstmeier, W., 2007. Superstition and belief as inevitable by-products of an adaptive learning strategy. Human Nature 18(1), 3546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bednarik, R., 2008. Cupules. Rock Art Research 25, 61100.Google Scholar
Bernieri, F. J. & Rosenthal, R., 1991. Interpersonal coordination: behaviour matching and interactional synchrony, in Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behaviour, eds. Feldman, R. S. & Rimé, B.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 401–32.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. & Pellegrini, A. D., 2002. The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouzouggar, A., Barton, N., Vanhaeren, M., d’Errico, F., Collcutt, S., Higham, T. et al. 2007. 82,000-year-old shell beads from North Africa and implications for the origins of modern human behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104(24), 9964–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, S. & Dissanayake, E., 2009. The arts are more than aesthetics: neuroaesthetics as narrow aesthetics, in Neuroaesthetics, eds. Skov, M. & Vartanian, O.. Amityville, NY: Baywood, 4357.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M., 2005. The Genesis of Animal Play: Testing the Limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrill, R., 2010. The primacy of movement in art making. Teaching Artist Journal 8, 216–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caporael, L. R., 1997. The evolution of truly social cognition: the core configuration model. Personality and Social Psychology Review 1, 276–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, C. S., 1998. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology 23, 779818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. & Altemus, M., 1999. Integrative functions of lactational hormones in social behaviour and stress management, in The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 361–71.Google Scholar
Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B., (eds.), 1999. The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Charmove, A. S. & Anderson, J. R., 1989. Examining environmental enrichment, in Housing, Care and Psychological Well Being of Captive and Laboratory Animals, ed. Segal, E. F.. Park Ridge, NJ: Noyes Publications, 183202.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A., 1999. The chameleon effect: the perception-behaviour link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76, 893910.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, N. S., Bussey, T. J. & Dickinson, A., 2003. Can animals recall the past and plan for the future? Nature Reviews Neuroscience 4, 685–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., 2000. Consider the source: the evolution of adaptations for decoupling and metarepresentation, in Metarepresentations, ed. Sperber, D.. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 53115.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., 1994. Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York: Grosset Putnam.Google Scholar
d’Errico, F., Verhaeren, M., Barton, N., Bouzouggar, A., Mienis, H., Richter, D. et al. 2009. Additional evidence on the use of personal ornaments in the Middle Paleolithic of North Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106(38), 16051–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Dissanayake, E., 1979. An ethological view of ritual and art in human evolutionary history. Leonardo 12(1), 2731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 1988. What Is Art For? Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 1999. Antecedents of the temporal arts in early mother–infant interaction, in The Origins of Music, eds. Wallin, N. L., Merker, B. & Brown, S.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 389410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2000. Art and Intimacy: How the Arts Began. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2001. Becoming Homo aestheticus: sources of aesthetic imagination in mother–infant interactions. SubStance 30(1,2), 85103.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2006. Ritual and ritualization: musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals, in Music and Manipulation: On the Social Uses and Social Control of Music, eds. Brown, S. & Volgsten, U.. Oxford: Berghahn, 3157.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E., 2008. The arts after Darwin: does art have an origin and adaptive function?, in World Art Studies: Exploring Concepts and Approaches, eds. Zijlmans, K. & Damme, W. van. Amsterdam: Valiz, 241–63.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2016). Mark-making as a human behavior, in Darwin’s Bridge: Uniting the Humanities and Social Sciences, eds. Carroll, J., McAdams, D. P., & Wilson, E. O.. New York: Oxford University Press, 101–30.Google Scholar
Donald, M., 1991. Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Cognition and Culture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dugatkin, L. A., 1997. Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M., Kaskatis, K., MacDonald, I. & Barra, V., 2012. Performance of music elevates pain threshold and positive affect: implications for the evolutionary function of music. Evolutionary Psychology 10(4): 688702.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 1971. Love and Hate: The Natural History of Behaviour Patterns, translated by Geoffrey Strachan. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 1989. Human Ethology, translated by Pauline Wiessner-Larsen and Anette Heunemann. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P., 1991. Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories of emotion, in International Review of Studies of Emotion, vol. 1, ed. Strongman, K.. New York: Wiley, 143–61.Google Scholar
Falk, D., 2004. Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: whence motherese? Behavioural and Brain Sciences 27(4), 491503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falk, D., 2009. Finding Our Tongues: Mothers, Infants & The Origin of Language. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Feierman, J. R. (ed.), 2009. The Biology of Religious Behaviour: The Evolutionary Origins of Faith and Religion. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.Google Scholar
Fein, S., 1993. First Drawings: Genesis of Visual Thinking. Pleasant Hill, CA: Exelrod Press.Google Scholar
Flinn, M. V., Quinlan, R. J., Decker, S. A., Turner, M. T. & England, B. G., 1996. Male-female differences in effects of parental absence on glucocorticoid stress response. Human Nature 7(2), 125–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flinn, M. V. & Ward, C. V., 2005. Evolution of the social child, in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, eds. Ellis, B. & Bjorklund, D.. London: Guilford Press, 1944.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., 2005. Folk knowledge and academic learning, in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, eds. Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D.F.. New York: Guilford, 493519.Google Scholar
Gianino, A. & Tronick, E. Z., 1988. The mutual regulation model: the infant’s self and interactive regulation and coping and defensive capacities, in Stress and Coping, eds. Field, T., McCabe, P. M. & Schneiderman, N.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 4768.Google Scholar
Guss, D. M., 1998. To Weave and Sing: Art, Symbol, and Narrative in the South American Rain Forest. Berkeley & Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R., 1995. Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review 102, 458–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hazan, C. & Zeifman, D., 1999. Pair bonds as attachments: evaluating the evidence, in Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, eds. Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R.. New York: Guilford, 336–54.Google Scholar
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirschbaum, C. & Ehlert, U., 2003. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress. Biological Psychiatry 54, 1389–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henshilwood, C. S., d’Errico, F., Yates, R., Jacobs, Z., Tribolo, C., Duller, G. A., et al. 2002. Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295(5558), 1278–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R., 1975. Biological Bases of Human Social Relationships. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
Hinde, R., 1982. Ethology: Its Nature and Relations With Other Sciences. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hofer, M. A., 1987. Early social relationships: a psychobiologist’s view. Child Development 58, 633–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmes, E. A., Brewin, C. R. & Hennessy, R. G., 2004. Trauma films, information processing, and intrusive memory development. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133(1), 322.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huether, G., Doering, S., Rueger, U. & Ruether, E., 1996. Psychic stress and neuronal plasticity: an expanded model of the stress reaction processes as basis for the understanding of adaptive processes in the central nervous system. Zeitschrift für Psychosomatische Medizin und Psychoanalyse 42, 107–27.Google Scholar
Huxley, J., 1914. The courtship habits of the great crested grebe (Podiceps cristatus) together with a discussion of the evolution of courtship in birds. Journal of the Linnean Society of London: Zoology 53, 253–92.Google Scholar
Huxley, J., 1966. Introduction, in A Discussion on Ritualisation of Behaviour in Animals and Man, organized by Sir Julian Huxley. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B. Biological Sciences 772, 249–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaptchuk, T. J., Kerr, C. E. & Zanger, A., 2009. Placebo controls, exorcisms, and the devil. Lancet 374 (9697), 1234–5. (10 Oct.).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kavanagh, D. J., Andrade, J. & May, J., 2005. Imaginary relish and exquisite torture: the elaborated intrusion theory of desire. Psychological Review 112(2), 446–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kellogg, R., 1970. Analyzing Children’s Art. Palo Alto, CA: Mayfield.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P. & Edwards, K., 1993. Beyond simple pessimism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64, 740–52.Google ScholarPubMed
King, B. J., 2004. The Dynamic Dance: Nonvocal Communication in African Great Apes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koelsch, S., Offermanns, K. & Franzke, P., 2010. Music in the treatment of affective disorders: an exploratory investigation of a new method for music-therapeutic research. Music Perception 27(4), 307–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leakey, R., 1994. The Origin of Humankind. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Leslie, A. M., 1987. Pretense and representation: origins of ‘theory of mind’. Psychological Review 94, 412–26.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., 1993. Pretend play skills and the child’s theory of mind. Child Development 64(2), 348–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopreato, J., 1984. Human Nature and Biocultural Evolution. Boston, MA: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. Z., 1982. The Foundations of Ethology: The Principal Ideas and Discoveries in Animal Behaviour, translated by K. Z. Lorenz and R. W. Kickert. (Original German publication 1981). New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1922. Argonauts of the Western Pacific. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1927. The life of culture, in Culture: The Diffusion Controversy, eds. Smith, G. E., Malinowski, B., Spinden, H. J. & Goldenweiser, A. New York: Norton, 2646.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B., 1948. Magic, Science, and Religion. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. Original publication 1925.Google Scholar
Mead, M. 1976. Growing Up in New Guinea. New York: Morrow. Original publication 1930.Google Scholar
Meyer-Holzapfel, M., 1956. Das Spiel bei Säugetieren. Handbuch der Zoologie 8, 126.Google Scholar
Miller, W. B. & Rodgers, J. L., 2001. The Ontogeny of Human Bonding Systems: Evolutionary Origins, Neural Bases, and Psychological Manifestations. Boston, MA and Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mithen, S., 2005. The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body. London: Weidenfeld and Nicolson.Google Scholar
Morris, D., 1957. ‘Typical intensity’ and its relation to the problem of ritualisation, Behaviour 11, 12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. E. & Panksepp, J., 1998. Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews 22, 437–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakley, K. P., 1971. Fossil shell observed by Acheulian Man. Antiquity 47, 5960.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., 1998. Affective Neuroscience: The Foundation of Animal and Human Emotions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Biven, L., 2012. The Archaeology of Mind: Neuroevolutionary Origins of Human Emotions. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J., Nelson, E. & Bekkedal, M., 1999. Brain systems for the mediation of social separation-distress and social-reward: evolutionary antecedents and neuropeptide intermediaries, in The Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C. S., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 222–41.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. & Bjorklund, D. F., 2004. The ontogeny and phylogeny of children’s object and fantasy play. Human Nature 15(1), 2343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips-Silver, J., Aktipis, C. A. & Bryant, G. A., 2010. The ecology of entrainment: foundations of coordinated rhythmic movement. Music Perception 28(1), 314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips-Silver, J. & Keller, P. E.. 2012. Searching for roots of entrainment and joint action in early musical interactions. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 6(26), 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raby, C. R., Alexis, D. M., Dickinson, A. & Clayton, N. S., 2007. Planning for the future by Western scrub-jays. Nature 445, 919–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radcliffe-Brown, A. R., 1952. Religion and society, in Structure and Function in Primitive Society. Glencoe, IL: Free Press, 153–77. Original publication in Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (1945) 75(1–2), 33–43.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A., 1999. Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity. Cambridge and New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, K. R., 1992. The evolution of modern human childbirth. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 35, 89134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M., 1992. Neuroendocrinology of the stress response, in Behavioural Endocrinology, eds. Becker, J. R., Breedlove, S. M. & Crews, D.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 287324.Google Scholar
Schechner, R., 2002. Performance Studies: An Introduction. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Schiefenhövel, W., 2009. Explaining the inexplicable: traditional and syncretistic religiosity in Melanesia, in The Biological Evolution of Religious Mind and Behaviour, eds. Voland, E. & Schiefenhövel, W.. Berlin: Springer, 143–64.Google Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Hazan, C. & Bradshaw, D., 1988. Love as attachment: the integration of three behavioral systems, in The Anatomy of Love, eds. Sternberg, R. J. & Barnes, M.. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, W., 1977. The Behavior of Communicating: An Evolutionary Approach. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. 1977. The First Relationship. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. & Corballis, M.C., 1997. Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs 123, 133–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Tambiah, S. J., 1979. A performative approach to ritual. Proceedings of the British Academy, London LXV, 113–69. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, S., 1992. The Tending Instinct: How Nurturing Is Essential to Who We Are and How We Live. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., 2007. Social support, in Foundations of Health Psychology, eds. H. S. Friedman & R. C. Silver. New York: Oxford University Press, 145–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Burklund, L. J., Eisenberger, N. I., Lehman, B. J., Hilmeet, C. J. & Lieberman, M. D., 2008. Neural bases of moderation of cortisol stress responses by psychosocial resources. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95(1), 197211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thieme, H., 1997. Lower Paleolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature 385, 807–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N., 1952. ‘Derived’ activities: their causation, biological significance, origin and emancipation during evolution. Quarterly Review of Biology 17, 132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tinbergen, N., 1959. Comparative studies of the behaviour of gulls (Laridae): a progress report. Behaviour 15, 170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonkinson, R., 1978. The Mardudjara Aborigines: Living the Dream in Australia’s Desert. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L., 2001. Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction, and the arts. SubStance 30(1,2), 627.Google Scholar
Turner, V., 1969. The Ritual Process: Structure and Anti-Structure. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Uvnäs-Moberg, K., 1999. Physiological and endocrine effects of social contact, in The Integrative Neurobiology of Affiliation, eds. Carter, C., Lederhendler, I. & Kirkpatrick, B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 245–61.Google Scholar
Van Gennep, A., 1960. The Rites of Passage. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul. Original work published in 1908.Google Scholar
Vanhaeren, M., d'Errico, F., Stringer, C., James, S. L., Todd, J. A., & H. K. Mienis. 2006. Middle paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312 (5781), 1785–8.Google Scholar
Watanabe, J. M. & Smuts, B. B., 1999. Explaining religion without explaining it away: trust, truth, and the evolution of cooperation in Roy A. Rappaport’s ‘The obvious aspects of ritual’. American Anthropologist 101, 98112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whybrow, P., 1984. Contributions from neuroendocrinology, in Approaches to Emotion, eds. Scherer, K. & Ekman, P.. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 5972.Google Scholar
Yellen, J. E., Brooks, A. S., Cornelissen, E., Mehlman, M. J. & Stewart, K., 1995. A Middle Stone Age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire. Science 268 (28 April), 553–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×