Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T04:19:35.181Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Measuring executive function in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: what is ecologically valid?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2011

Ilona Roth
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Payam Rezaie
Affiliation:
The Open University, Milton Keynes
Get access

Summary

Clinical experience and anecdotal written accounts suggest that school-age children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have difficulties which can be described as ‘executive dysfunction’. Problems with organisation, planning and task completion impede academic achievement and cause disruption in daily routine. The authors review research of executive function in this population and conclude that clinicians will find little in the scientific literature to guide them in neuropsychological assessment and remediation. They describe their study of 23 clinic-referred children (18 boys, 5 girls; mean age of 9) illustrating the challenges facing clinicians who would measure executive function. Tests of executive function (including the NEPSY and the BADS-C) were administered. Parent and Teacher questionnaires (DEX-C, BRIEF and VABS) were completed. Scores on tests of executive function and other areas of cognition were found to be in the average or above average range. In contrast, responses on both teacher and parent questionnaires indicated significant executive dysfunction. Parents' responses on the BRIEF and on the DEX-C were not correlated with teacher responses on the BRIEF. The authors consider the importance of a “halo effect” on questionnaire responses and challenge the notion that questionnaire measures have more ecological validity than laboratory measures. Suggestions for future research include observation, interviews and graded modification of the testing environment.

Introduction

Not enough attention is given to identifying individual neuropsychological deficits of children with ASD in the clinical setting with the aim of remediation despite being recommended by the National Autism Plan for Children (2003).

Type
Chapter
Information
Researching the Autism Spectrum
Contemporary Perspectives
, pp. 347 - 363
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbach, T.M., Bird, H.R., Canino, G., et al. (1990). Epidemiological comparisons of Puerto Rican and U.S. mainland children: parent, teacher, and self-reports. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29: 84–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alcantara, J.I., Weisblatt, E.J., Moore, B.C., Bolton, P.F. (2004). Speech-in-noise perception in high-functioning individuals with Autism or Asperger's Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45: 1107–1114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, V. (1998). Assessment of executive function in children. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 8: 319–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, I.S. (2004). Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Child. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S.B., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V., Rutherford, M. (1999). A mathematician, a physicist, and a computer scientist with Asperger Syndrome: performance on folk psychology and folk physics tests. Neurocase, 5: 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Betts, S.W., Betts, D.E., Gerber-Eckard, L.N. (2007). Asperger Syndrome in the Inclusive Classroom: Advice and Strategies for Teachers. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Bogte, H., Flamma, B., Meere, J., Engeland, H. (2009). Divided attention capacity in adults with autism spectrum disorders and without intellectual disability. Autism, 13: 229–243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burgess, P.W., Alderman, N., Evans, J., Emslie, H., Wilson, B.A. (1998). The ecological validity of tests of executive function. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 4: 547–558.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Channon, S., Charman, T., Heap, J., Crawford, S., Rios, P. (2001). Real-life-type problem-solving in Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31: 461–469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DuPaul, G., Power, T., Anastropoulos, A., Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklist, Norms and Clinical Interpretation. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Emslie, H., Wilson, C., Burden, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Wilson, B. (2003). Behavioral Assessment of Dysexecutive Syndrome for Children (BADS-C). Bury St. Edmunds, England: Thames Valley Test Company.Google Scholar
Eslinger, P.J. (1996). Conceptualizing, describing, and measuring components of executive function. In Attention, Memory and Executive Function. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes, pp. 367–395.Google Scholar
Gilotty, L., Kenworthy, L., Sirian, L., Black, D.O., Wagner, A.E. (2002). Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 8: 241–248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G., Isquith, P., Guy, S., Kenworthy, L. (2002). Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF). Odessa, Florida; Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Hill, E.L. (2004). Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8: 26–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, E.L., Bird, C.M. (2006). Executive processes in Asperger Syndrome: patterns of performance in a multiple case series. Neuropsychologia, 44: 2822–2835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, A. (2006). Children behaving badly – a case of misunderstanding? The development of a CAMHS based child neuropsychology service. The Psychologist, 19: 494–495.Google Scholar
Jackson, D.A., King, A.R. (2004). Gender differences in the effects of oppositional behavior on teacher ratings of ADHD symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32: 215–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobsen, P. (2005). Understanding How Asperger Children and Adolescents Think and Learn. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Kenworthy, L.E., Black, D.O., Wallace, G.L., et al. (2005). Disorganization: the forgotten executive dysfunction in high-functioning autism (HFA) spectrum disorders. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28: 809–827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleinhans, N., Akshoomoff, N., Delis, D.C. (2005). Executive functions in Autism and Asperger's Disorder: flexibility, fluency, and inhibition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27: 379–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korkman, M., Kirk, U., Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment. San Antonio, Texas: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Lezak, M. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd edn. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liss, M., Fein, D., Allen, D., et al. (2001). Executive functioning in high-functioning children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 42: 261–270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGee, R., Williams, S., Silva, P.A. (1987). A comparison of girls and boys with teacher-identified problems of attention. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26: 711–717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, D.L. (1995). Interrater reliability of behavioural observations on NEPSY. Unpublished Master's Thesis, Trinity University, San Antonio, TX.
,National Autism Plan for Children (NAPC). (2003). National Initiative for Autism: Screening and Assessment (NAISA) Guidelines. London: National Autistic Society.Google Scholar
Pennington, B.F., Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 37: Jan, 51–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sainsbury, C. (2000). Martian in the Playground. Bristol: Lucky Duck Publishing Ltd.Google Scholar
Sergeant, J.A., Geurts, H., Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioral Brain Research, 130: 3–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., Burgess, P. (1991). Higher order cognitive impairments and frontal lobe lesions in man. In Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sparrow, S., Balla, D., Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales – Survey Form. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×