Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T23:06:18.414Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Creative remembering

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Ulric Neisser
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Robyn Fivush
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Get access

Summary

Research on nonliterate societies reveals striking examples of “amnesia”: Prior events or beliefs that contradict current ideas and values are either erased from the collective memory or altered so as to be consistent with present understandings (Goody & Watt, 1968; Henige, 1980; Ong, 1982; Packard, 1980). For example, when the British arrived in Ghana in the early part of this century, they found that the state of Gonja was divided into seven territories, each ruled by its own chief (Goody & Watt, 1968). When British authorities asked them to explain their system, the Gonja revealed that the founder of their state, Ndewura Jakpa, had fathered seven sons. Jakpa divided the land so that each son ruled one territory. The British preserved this account of the history of Gonja in their written records. Shortly after the British arrived, two of the seven states in Gonja disappeared as a result of changes in boundaries. Sixty years later, oral historians again recorded the myths of state. In the updated version, Ndewura Jakpa begot only five sons; the Gonja made no mention of the founders of the two territories that had vanished from the scene. Oral historians have observed similar instances of forgetting or altering inconvenient aspects of the past in many other nonliterate societies (Goody & Watt, 1968).

In literate societies, individuals also revise history, especially in response to changing knowledge and political regimes (Greenwald, 1980). People interpret the past in terms of the present and therefore “every generation rewrites its history” (Mead, 1929/1964, p. 351). Because the earlier records are often preserved, people may notice differences between current and previous accounts of the past (Goody & Watt, 1968; Ong, 1982).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Remembering Self
Construction and Accuracy in the Self-Narrative
, pp. 205 - 235
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×