Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 August 2016
Introduction
Postchiasmatic brain lesions are reported in association with a variety of visual disorders after stroke, including cortical blindness, visual field disorders, achromatopsia, akinetopsia, visual agnosia, prosopagnosia, visuospatial neglect and Balint's syndrome (for review see Grüsser and Landis, 1991). This chapter is devoted to the rehabilitation of these syndromes in the context of acquired brain lesions in adults who had normal vision and normal visuocognitive performance prior to the stroke.
As much as two-thirds of the hemispheres is devoted to visual analysis (Zilles and Clarke, 1997) and, therefore, it is not surprising that 20–40% of patients with stroke have visual disorders (Hier et al., 1983). Visual disorders often have very debilitating effects in everyday life and specific techniques for their rehabilitation have been developed (Zihl, 2000), often based on the understanding of visual processing derived from recent work in humans or in non-human primates.
Neurobiological basis of visual perception
Experimental work since the early 1970s has changed radically our understanding of visual processing. Three issues are of great importance to visual rehabilitation. First, although visual information is relayed to the cortex primarily via the retinogeniculo–primary visual cortex route, alternative, smaller routes have been identified via the pulvinar or the superior colliculus. This is highly relevant to the understanding of preserved capacities in those with brain lesions, such as blindsight (Stoerig and Cowey, 1997) or interhemispheric transfer of visual information following callosotomy (Clarke et al., 2000). Second, the visual association cortex contains a large number of specialized visual areas. This was shown to be the case in non-human primates, where over 30 visual areas were identified (Felleman and van Essen, 1991). Activation studies indicate that human extrastriate visual cortex also contains functionally defined visual areas, some of which are highly specialized. This is the case for area V4, specialized for color perception (Zeki, 1990), and for area V5-MT, specialized for motion perception (Zeki, 1991). Selective deficits, such as achromatopsia or akinetopsia, are currently interpreted as the result of damage to these specialized areas. Third, the information flow within extrastriate visual cortex is directed along two streams: the ventral stream, subserving recognition and involving the inferior temporal cortex (the “What” stream), and the dorsal stream, subserving spatial aspects and involving the parietal cortex (the “Where” stream) (Mishkin et al., 1983).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.