Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pjpqr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-04T20:50:50.992Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Select Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2015

Yellowlees Douglas
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Reader's Brain
How Neuroscience Can Make You a Better Writer
, pp. 199 - 213
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, Richard C., Spiro, Rand J., and Anderson, Mark C.. (1978). “Schemata as Scaffolding for the Representation of Information in Connected Discourse,” American Educational Research Journal 15/3: 433–440.Google Scholar
Anthony, Ted. (2007). Chasing the Rising Sun: The Journey of an American Song. New York: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Atkinson, R. C. and Shiffrin, R. M.. (1968). “Human Memory: A Proposed System and Its Control Processes,” in Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. New York: Academic Press: 89–195.Google Scholar
Baddeley, Alan. (2004). Your Memory: A User's Guide. Buffalo, NY: Firefly Books.Google Scholar
Baddeley, Alan D. and Hitch, Graham. (1993). “The Recency Effect: Implicit Learning with Explicit Retrieval?Memory & Cognition 21/2: 146–155.Google Scholar
Baddeley, Alan D., Thomson, Neil, and Buchanan, Mary. (1975). “Word Length and the Structure of Short-Term Memory,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 14/6: 575–589.Google Scholar
Bain, Alexander. (1890). English Composition and Rhetoric. Enlarged edn. London: Longmans, Green, and Company.Google Scholar
Bazerman, Charles. (2003). “Speech Acts, Genres, and Activity Systems: How Texts Organize Activity and People,” in Bazerman, Charles and Prior, Paul (eds.), What Writing Does and How It Does It: An Introduction to Analyzing Texts and Textual Practices. New York: Routledge: 309–339.Google Scholar
Berk, Robert N. and Whalen, Elizabeth. (1992). “Impediments to Clarity: An Annotated Glossary of Rhetorical Pratfalls and Pitfalls,” AJR: American Journal of Roentgenology 159/5: 1115–1121.Google Scholar
Bernal, Byron and Ardila, Alfredo. (2009). “The Role of the Arcuate Fasciculus in Conduction Aphasia,” Brain 132/9: 2309–2316.Google Scholar
Billig, Michael. (2008). “The Language of Critical Discourse Analysis: The Case of Nominalization,” Discourse & Society 19/6: 783–800.Google Scholar
Black, John B. and Bower, Gordon H.. (1979). “Episodes as Chunks in Narrative Memory,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 18: 309–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornkessel, Ina, Schlesewsky, Matthias, and Friederici, Angela D.. (2002). “Beyond Syntax: Language-related Positivities Reflect the Revision of Hierarchies,” NeuroReport 13/3: 361–364.Google Scholar
Bornkessel, Ina, Zysset, Stefan, Friederici, Angela D., von Cramon, D. Yves, and Schlesewsky, Matthias. (2005). “Who Did What to Whom? The Neural Basis of Argument Hierarchies during Language Comprehension,” Neuroimage 26/1: 221–233.Google Scholar
Bower, Gordon H., Black, John B., and Turner, Terrence J.. (1979). “Scripts in Memory for Text,” Cognitive Psychology 11/2: 177–220.Google Scholar
Britt, M. Anne. (1994). “The Interaction of Referential Ambiguity and Argument Structure in the Parsing of Prepositional Phrases,” Journal of Memory and Language 33/2: 251–283.Google Scholar
Britton, Bruce K. (1994). “Understanding Expository Text: Building Mental Structures to Induce Insights,” in Gernsbacher, Morton Ann (ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic Press: 641–674.Google Scholar
Britton, James. (1982). “A Reader's Expectations,” in Pradl, Gordon M. (ed.), Prospect and Retrospect: Selected Essays of James Britton. Montclair, NJ: Boynton/Cook: 130–138.Google Scholar
Brown, Ann L. (1982). “Learning How to Learn from Reading,” in Langer, Judith A. and Smith-Burke, M. Trika (eds.), Reader Meets Author/Bridging the Gap: A Psycholinguistic and Sociolinguistic Perspective. Newark, DE: International Reading Association: 26–54.Google Scholar
Bruner, Jerome. (1986).Actual Minds, Possible Worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryson, Bill. (1990). The Mother Tongue: English and How It Got That Way. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Büchel, Christian, Price, Cathy, Frackowiak, R. S., and Friston, Karl. (1998). “Different Activation Patterns in the Visual Cortex of Late and Congenitally Blind Subjects,” Brain 121/3: 409–419.Google Scholar
Carpenter, Ronald H. (1999). Choosing Powerful Words: Eloquence That Works. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Carter, Rita. (1998). Mapping the Mind. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, Wallace L. (1974). “Language and Consciousness,” Language 50/1: 111–133.Google Scholar
Chang, Franklin, Dell, Gary S., Bock, Kathryn, and Griffin, Zenzi M.. (2000). “Structural Priming as Implicit Learning: A Comparison of Models of Sentence Production,” Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 29/2: 217–230.Google Scholar
Cirilo, Randolph K. (1981). “Referential Coherence and Text Structure in Story Comprehension,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20/3: 358–367.Google Scholar
Clanchy, Michael T. (1979). From Memory to Written Record 1066–1307. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. and Sengul, C. J.. (1979). “In Search of Referents for Nouns and Pronouns,” Memory & Cognition 7/1: 35–41.Google Scholar
Clifton, Charles, Frazier, Lyn, and Connine, Cynthia. (1984). “Lexical Expectations in Sentence Comprehension,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23/6: 696–708.Google Scholar
Clifton, Charles, Staub, Adrian, and Rayner, Keith. (2007). “Eye Movements in Reading Words and Sentences,” in van Gompel, Roger P. G. (ed.), Eye Movements: A Window on Mind and Brain. Amsterdam: Elsevier: 341–372.Google Scholar
Cobban, Alan B. (1975). The Medieval Universities: Their Development and Organisation. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Cordes, Jason L. (2002). “When Liability Can Attach for Remaining Passive: Construction of the Term ‘Disposal’ under Cercla: Crofton Ventures Limited Partnership v. G & H Partnership 1,” Missouri Environmental Law & Policy Review 9: 67–154.Google Scholar
Costello, Fintan J. and Keane, Mark T.. (2000). “Efficient Creativity: Constraint-guided Conceptual Combination,” Cognitive Science 24/2: 299–349.Google Scholar
Coulson, Seana, King, Jonathan W., and Kutas, Marta. (1998). “Expect the Unexpected: Event-related Brain Response to Morphosyntactic Violations,” Language & Cognitive Processes 13/1: 21–58.Google Scholar
Crowhurst, Marion. (1990). “Reading/Writing Relationships: An Intervention Study,” Canadian Journal of Education 15/2: 155–172.Google Scholar
Crystal, David. (2004). The Stories of English. New York: Overlook Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, Peter W. and Levine, Robert D.. (2001). “Stylistic Inversion in English: A Reconsideration,” Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 19/2: 283–310.Google Scholar
Cutrell, Edward and Guan, Zhiwei. (2007). “What Are You Looking For?: An Eye-Tracking Study of Information Usage in Web Search,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. New York: Association for Digital Machinery (ACM).Google Scholar
Daneman, Meredyth. (1982). “The Measurement of Reading Comprehension: How Not to Trade Construct Validity for Predictive Power,” Intelligence 6/4: 331–345.Google Scholar
Daneman, Meredyth and Carpenter, Patricia A. (1983). “Individual Differences in Integrating Information between and within Sentences,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 9/4: 561–584.Google Scholar
Davelaar, Eddy J., Goshen-Gottstein, Yonatan, Ashkenazi, Amir, Haarmann, Henk J., and Usher, Marius. (2005). “The Demise of Short-term Memory Revisited: Empirical and Computational Investigations of Recency Effects,” Psychological Review 112/1: 3–42.Google Scholar
Davis, Kirsten. (2005). “Persuasion through Organization: The Final Installment – Transitions,” Arizona Attorney: 31–33.Google Scholar
Dee-Lucas, Diana, Just, Marcel Adam, Carpenter, Patricia A., and Daneman, Meredyth. (1982). “What Eye Fixations Tell Us about the Time Course of Text Integration,” in Groner, Rudolf and Fraisse, Paul (eds.), Cognition and Eye Movements. Leipzig: North-Holland: 155–168.Google Scholar
Dehaene, Stanislas. (2009). Reading in the Brain: The Science and Evolution of a Human Invention. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
DeKay, Sam H. (2010). “Focus on Business Practices: Designing Email Messages for Corporate Readers: A Case Study of Effective and Ineffective Rhetorical Strategies at a Fortune 100 Company,” Business Communication Quarterly 73/1: 109–119.Google Scholar
Dermer, Marshall L., Lopez, Shannon L., and Messling, Paul A.. (2010). “Fluency Training a Writing Skill: Editing for Concision,” The Psychological Record 59/1: 3–20.Google Scholar
Diederich, Paul, French, John W., and Carlton, Sydell T.. (1961). “Factors in Judgments of Writing Quality,” Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service: ERIC ED 002 172.
Dor, Daniel. (2003). “On Newspaper Headlines as Relevance Optimizers,” Journal of Pragmatics 35/5: 695–721.Google Scholar
Duggan, Geoffrey B. and Payne, Stephen J.. (2011). “Skim Reading by Satisficing: Evidence from Eye Tracking,” in Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. New York: Association for Digital Machinery (ACM): 1141–1150.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, Elizabeth L. (1979). The Printing Press as an Agent of Change: Communications and Cultural Transformations in Early Modern Europe. 2 vols. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eisenstein, Elizabeth L. (1983). The Printing Revolution in Early Modern Europe. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Erasmus, Desiderius. (1978). Copia: Foundations of the Abundant Style (De Duplici Copia Verborum ac Rerum Commentarii Duo). Trans. Knott, Betty. In Thompson, Craig R. (ed.), Collected Works of Erasmus. Vol. xxiv. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Evans, Vyvyan. (2014). The Language Myth: Why Language Is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fahnestock, Jeanne. (1983). “Semantic and Lexical Coherence,” College Composition and Communication 34/4: 400–416.Google Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda. (2003). “The Misinterpretation of Noncanonical Sentences,” Cognitive Psychology 47: 164–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, Fernanda, Bailey, Karl G. D., and Ferraro, Vittoria. (2002). “Good-enough Representations in Language Comprehension,” Current Directions in Psychological Science 11/1: 11–15.Google Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda, and Clifton, Charles. (1986). “The Independence of Syntactic Processing,” Journal of Memory and Language 25: 348–368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda and Stacey, Janis. (2000). “The Misinterpretation of Passive Sentences,” unpublished manuscript.
Fischer, Steven Roger. (2005). A History of Reading. Amsterdam: Reaktion Books.Google Scholar
Fitzgerald, Jill and Shanahan, Timothy. (2000). “Reading and Writing Relations and Their Development,” Educational Psychologist 35/1: 39–50.Google Scholar
Fleischman, Suzanne. (1990). Tense and Narrativity: From Medieval Performance to Modern Fiction. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Flesch, Rudolf. (1974). The Art of Readable Writing. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Flesch, Rudolf. (1979). How to Write in Plain English: A Book for Lawyers and Consumers. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Fowler, H. W. (1965). A Dictionary of Modern English Usage. 2nd edn. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fox, Julia R., Park, Byungho, and Lang, Annie. (2007). “When Available Resources Become Negative Resources: The Effects of Cognitive Overload on Memory Sensitivity and Criterion Bias,” Communication Research 34/3: 277–296.Google Scholar
Frase, Lawrence T. (1969). “Paragraph Organization of Written Materials: The Influence of Conceptual Clustering upon the Level and Organization of Recall,” Journal of Educational Psychology 60/5: 394–401.Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn and Rayner, Keith. (1982). “Making and Correcting Errors during Sentence Comprehension: Eye Movements in the Analysis of Structurally Ambiguous Sentences,” Cognitive Psychology 14/2: 178–210.Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, Taft, Lori, Roeper, Tom, Clifton, Charles, and Ehrlich, Kate. (1984). “Parallel Structure: A Source of Facilitation in Sentence Comprehension,” Memory & Cognition 12/5: 421–430.Google Scholar
Garnham, Alan, Oakhill, Jane, and Johnson-Laird, P. N.. (1982). “Referential Continuity and the Coherence of Discourse,” Cognition 11/1: 29–46.Google Scholar
Gibson, Edward. (1998). “Linguistic Complexity: Locality of Syntactic Dependencies,” Cognition 68: 1–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glanzer, M. (1972). “Storage Mechanisms in Recall,” in Bower, G. H. and Spence, J. T. (eds.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. New York: Academic Press: 129–193.Google Scholar
Glenberg, Arthur M. and Swanson, Naomi G.. (1986). “A Temporal Distinctiveness Theory of Recency and Modality Effects,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 12/1: 3–15.Google Scholar
Gombrich, E. H. (1961). Art and Illusion: A Study in the Psychology of Pictorial Representation. New York: Bollingen Foundation.Google Scholar
Gordon, Peter C., Hendrick, Randall, and Levine, William H.. (2002). “Memory-load Interference in Syntactic Processing,” Psychological Science 13/5: 425–430.Google Scholar
Graesser, Arthur C., Millis, Keith K., and Zwaan, Rolf A.. (1997). “Discourse Comprehension,” Annual Review of Psychology 48/1: 163–189.Google Scholar
Graesser, Arthur C., Karnavat, Ashish B., Daniel, Frances K., Cooper, Elisa, Whitten, Shannon N., and Louwerse, Max. (2001). “A Computer Tool to Improve Questionnaire Design,” paper presented at the Funding Opportunity in Survey Research Seminar, June 11, Bureau of Labor Statistics, Washington, DC.
Graf, Peter, and Schacter, Daniel L.. (1985). “Implicit and Explicit Memory for New Associations in Normal and Amnesic Subjects,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 11/3: 501–518.Google Scholar
Greene, Steven B. and McKoon, Gail. (1995). “Telling Something We Can't Know: Experimental Approaches to Verbs Exhibiting Implicit Causality,” Psychological Science 6/5: 262–270.Google Scholar
Grice, H. Paul. (1975). “Logic and Conversation,” in Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L. (eds.), Speech Acts. New York: Academic Press: 41–58.Google Scholar
Gunning, Robert and Kallan, Richard A.. (1994). How to Take the Fog out of Business Writing. Chicago: Dartnell.Google Scholar
Hagoort, Peter, Brown, Colin, and Groothusen, Jolanda. (1993). “The Syntactic Positive Shift (SPS) as an ERP Measure of Syntactic Processing,” Language & Cognitive Processes 8/4: 439–483.Google Scholar
Hakala, Christopher M. and O'Brien, Edward J.. (1995). “Strategies for Resolving Coherence Breaks in Reading,” Discourse Processes 20/2: 167–185.Google Scholar
Handlin, Amy, Mosca, Joseph B., Forgione, Dana A., and Pitta, Dennis. (2003). “DTC Pharmaceutical Advertising: The Debate's not Over,” Journal of Consumer Marketing 20/3: 227–237.Google Scholar
Harwood, Nigel. (2005). “‘Nowhere Has Anyone Attempted … in This Article I Aim to Do Just That’: A Corpus-based Study of Self-Promotional I and We in Academic Writing across Four Disciplines,” Journal of Pragmatics 37/8: 1207–1231.Google Scholar
Heider, Fritz and Simmel, Marianne. (1944). “An Experimental Study of Apparent Behavior,” American Journal of Psychology 57/2: 243–259.Google Scholar
Heilman, Kenneth M., Leon, Susan A., and Rosenbek, John C.. (2004). “Affective Aprosodia from a Medial Frontal Stroke,” Brain and language 89/3: 411–416.Google Scholar
Herbert, A. S. (1967). Historical Catalogue of Printed Editions of the English Bible 1525–1961. New York: American Bible Society.Google Scholar
Higgins, Derrick, Burstein, Jill, Marcu, Daniel, and Gentile, Claudia. (2004). “Evaluating Multiple Aspects of Coherence in Student Essays,” paper presented at the Higher Learning/Teaching-NAACL, Boston, MA. Available as of 2014 via download at www.ets.org/Media/Research/pdf/erater_higgins_dis_coh.pdf.
Horwitz, B., Rumsey, J. M., and Donohue, B. C.. (1998). “Functional Connectivity of the Angular Gyrus in Reading and Dyslexia,” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95: 8939–8944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyland, Ken. (2004). “Disciplinary Interactions: Metadiscourse in L2 Postgraduate Writing,” Journal of Second Language Writing 13/2: 133–151.Google Scholar
Hyland, Ken and Tse, Polly. (2004). “Metadiscourse in Academic Writing: A Reappraisal,” Applied Linguistics 25/2: 156–177.Google Scholar
Just, Marcel Adam and Carpenter, Patricia A.. (1980). “A Theory of Reading: From Eye Fixations to Comprehension,” Psychological Review 87/4: 329–354.Google Scholar
Just, Marcel Adam, Carpenter, Patricia A., Keller, Timothy A., Eddy, William F., and Thulborn, Keith R.. (1996). “Brain Activation Modulated by Sentence Comprehension,” Science 274: 114–116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kieras, David E. (1978). “Good and Bad Structure in Simple Paragraphs: Effects on Apparent Theme, Reading Time, and Recall,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 17/1: 13–28.Google Scholar
King, Jonathan and Just, Marcel Adam. (1991). “Individual Differences in Syntactic Processing: The Role of Working Memory,” Journal of Memory and Language 30/5: 580–602.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter. (1988). “The Role of Knowledge in Discourse Comprehension: A Construction-Integration Model,” Psychological Review 95/2: 163–182.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter. (2002). “On the Notions of Theme and Topic in Psychological Process Models of Text Comprehension,” Thematics: Interdisciplinary Studies 3: 157–170.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter and van Dijk, Teun A.. (1978). “Toward a Model of Text Comprehension and Production,” Psychological Review 85/5: 363–394.Google Scholar
Kintsch, Walter and Vipond, Douglas. (1979). “Reading Comprehension and Readability in Educational Practice and Psychological Theory,” in Nilsson, Lars-Göran (ed.), Perspectives on Memory Research: Essays in Honor of Uppsala University's 500th Anniversary. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: 329–365.Google Scholar
Klee, Hilary and Legge, David. (1976). “Estimates of Concreteness and Other Indices for 200 Transitive Verbs,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 2/4: 497–507.Google Scholar
Knoblauch, C. H. and Brannon, Lil. (1984). Rhetorical Traditions and the Teaching of Writing. Portsmouth, NH: Boynton/Cook.Google Scholar
Landauer, Thomas. (1995). The Trouble with Computers. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lebovits, Gerald. (2006). “The Department of Redundancy Department: Concision and Succinctness – Part II,” New York State Bar Association Journal 78/7: 44–54.Google Scholar
Lepore, Jill. (2014). “Away from My Desk,” The New Yorker, May 12: 72–74.Google Scholar
Leslie, Alan and Keeble, Stephanie. (1987). “Do Six-month-old Infants Perceive Causality?Cognition 25: 265–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieberman, Philip. (2001). “Human Language and Our Reptilian Brain: The Subcortical Bases of Speech, Syntax, and Thought,” Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 44/1: 32–51.Google Scholar
Limaye, Mohan R. and Cherry, Roger D.. (1987). “Pragmatics, ‘Situated’ Language, and Business Communication,” Journal of Business and Technical Communication 1/1: 68–88.Google Scholar
Locker, Kitty O. (1987). “‘As Per Your Request’: A History of Business Jargon,” Journal of Business and Technical Communication 1/1: 27–47.Google Scholar
Lorch, Robert F. and Lorch, Elizabeth P.. (1985). “Topic Structure Representation and Text Recall,” Journal of Education Psychology 77/2: 137–148.Google Scholar
Luchins, Abraham S. (1958). “Definitiveness of Impression and Primacy–Recency in Communications,” Journal of Social Psychology 48/2: 275–290.Google Scholar
Lupker, Stephen J. (1984). “Semantic Priming without Association: A Second Look,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23/6: 709–733.Google Scholar
Mackay, Wendy E. (2000). “Responding to Cognitive Overload: Co-adaptation between Users and Technology,” Intellectica 30/1: 177–193.Google Scholar
Manguel, Alberto. (1996). A History of Reading. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Mason, Robert A., Just, Marcel Adam, Keller, Timothy A., and Carpenter, Patricia A.. (2003). “Ambiguity in the Brain: What Brain Imaging Reveals About the Processing of Syntactically Ambiguous Sentences,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 29/6: 1319–1338.Google Scholar
Masson, Michael E. (1982). “Cognitive Processes in Skimming Stories,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 8/5: 400–417.Google Scholar
Matsuda, Paul Kei and Tardy, Christine M.. (2007). “Voice in Academic Writing: The Rhetorical Construction of Author Identity in Blind Manuscript Review,” English for Specific Purposes 26/2: 235–249.Google Scholar
Mayer, Richard E. (1976). “Integration of Information during Problem Solving due to a Meaningful Context of Learning,” Memory & Cognition 4/5: 603–608.Google Scholar
McCarthy, Philip M., Renner, Adam M., Duncan, Michael G., Duran, Nicholas D., Lightman, Erin J., and McNamara, Danielle S.. (2008). “Identifying Topic Sentencehood,” Behavior Research Methods 40/3: 647–664.Google Scholar
McCrum, Robert, Cran, William, and MacNeil, Robert. (1992). The Story of English. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
McNamara, Danielle S., Crossley, Scott A., and McCarthy, Philip M.. (2010). “Linguistic Features of Writing Quality,” Written Communication 27/1: 57–86.Google Scholar
McWhorter, John. (2001). The Power of Babel: A Natural History of Language. New York: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Michael, Erica B., Keller, Timothy A., Carpenter, Patricia A., and Just, Marcel Adam. (2001). “fMRI Investigation of Sentence Comprehension by Eye and by Ear: Modality Fingerprints on Cognitive Processes,” Human Brain Mapping 13: 239–252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michotte, A. (1963). The Perception of Causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Miller, George A. (1956). “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: Some Limits on Our Capacity for Processing Information,” Psychological Review 63: 81–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, James R. and Kintsch, Walter. (1980). “Recall and Readability of Short Prose Passages.” paper presented at the American Educational Research Association Conference, Boston, MA. April 7–11: 32pp.
Miyake, Akira, Just, Marcel Adam, and Carpenter, Patricia A.. (1994). “Working Memory Constraints on the Resolution of Lexical Ambiguity: Maintaining Multiple Interpretations in Neutral Contexts,” Journal of Memory and Language 33/2: 175–202.Google Scholar
Moore, Gordon E. (1965). “Cramming More Components onto Integrated Circuits,” Electronics, April 19: 114–117.Google Scholar
Moreno, Roxana and Mayer, Richard E.. (2000). “A Learner-centered Approach to Multimedia Explanations: Deriving Instructional Design Principles from Cognitive Theory,” Interactive Multimedia Electronic Journal of Computer-enhanced Learning 2/2: 12–20.Google Scholar
Murdock, Bennet B. (1962). “The Serial Position Effect of Free Recall,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 64/5: 482–488.Google Scholar
Murray, John D. and McGlone, Chadd. (1997). “Topic Overviews and Processing of Topic Structure,” Journal of Educational Psychology 89/2: 251–261.Google Scholar
Nell, Victor. (1988). Lost in a Book: The Psychology of Reading for Pleasure. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicholas, Serge. (1998). “Perceptual and Conceptual Priming of Individual Words in Coherent Texts,” Memory 6/6: 643–663.Google Scholar
Nielsen, Jakob. (2006). “F-shaped Pattern for Reading Web Content,” Alertbox: Current Issues in Web Usability. Available as of 2014 from http://www.useit.com/alertbox/reading_patternhtml.Nishimura, Hiroshi, Hashikawa, Kazuo, Doi, Katsumi, Iwaki, Takako, Watanabe, Yoshiyuki, Kusuoka, Hideo, Nishimura, Tsunehiko, and Kubo, Takeshi. (1999). “Sign Language ‘Heard’ in the Auditory Cortex,” Nature 397: 116.Google Scholar
Noordman, Leo, Vonk, Wietske, and Kempff, Henk J.. (1992). “Causal Inferences during the Reading of Expository Texts,” Journal of Memory and Language 31/5: 573–590.Google Scholar
Norman, Suzanne, Kemper, Susan, Kynette, Donna, Cheung, Hintat, and Anagnopoulos, Cheryl. (1991). “Syntactic Complexity and Adults’ Running Memory Span,” Journal of Gerontology 46/6: 346–351.Google Scholar
Olson, David R. (1991). “Literacy and Objectivity: The Rise of Modern Science,” in Olson, David R. and Torrance, Nancy (eds.), Literacy and Orality. New York: Cambridge University Press: 149–164.Google Scholar
Olson, David R. and Filby, Nikola. (1972). “On the Comprehension of Active and Passive Sentences,” Cognitive Psychology 3/3: 361–481.Google Scholar
Ong, Walter J. (1982). Orality and Literacy: The Technologizing of the Word. New York: Methuen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orwell, George. (1986). “Politics and the English Language,” in Abrams, M. H. (ed.), The Norton Anthology of English Literature. London: W. W. Norton: 2260–2270.Google Scholar
Paivio, Allan. (1990). Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, Aniruddh D. (2010). Music, Language, and the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Paulesu, Eraldo, Frith, Christopher D., and Frackowiak, Richard S. J.. (1993). “The Neural Correlates of the Verbal Component of Working Memory,” Nature 362/25: 342–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, Olaf. (1997). The First Universities: Studium Generale and the Origins of University Education in Europe. Trans. North, Richard. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pennebaker, James W. (2011). The Secret Life of Pronouns: What Our Words Say about Us. New York: Bloomsbury Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, Charles A. (1999). “Comprehending Written Language: A Blueprint of the Reader,” in Brown, C. M. and Hagoort, P. (eds.), The Neurocognition of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press: 167–208.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. and Branigan, Holly P.. (1998). “The Representation of Verbs: Evidence from Syntactic Priming in Language Production,” Journal of Memory and Language 39: 633–651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. and Ferreira, Victor S.. (2008). “Structural Priming: A Critical Review,” Psychological Bulletin 134/3: 427–459.Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. and Garrod, Simon. (2013). “Forward Models and Their Implications for Production, Comprehension, and Dialogue,” Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36/4: 377–392.Google Scholar
Pinker, Steven. (1994). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Generates Language. New York: Harper.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plato, . (1973). Phaedrus. Trans. Hamilton, Walter. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pluchino, Stefano, Quattrini, Angelo, Brambilla, Elena, Gritti, Angela, Salani, Giuliana, Dina, Giorgia, Galli, Rossella et al. (2003).“Injection of Adult Neurospheres Induces Recovery in a Chronic Model of Multiple Sclerosis,” Nature 422: 688–694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S. (2011). The Tell-Tale Brain: A Neuroscientist's Quest for What Makes Us Human. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Rayner, Keith, Carlson, Marcia, and Frazier, Lyn. (1983). “The Interaction of Syntax and Semantics during Sentence Processing: Eye Movements in the Analysis of Semantically Biased Sentences,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22/3: 358–374.Google Scholar
Rayner, Keith, Foorman, Barbara R., Perfetti, Charles A., Pesetsky, David, and Seidenberg, Mark S.. (2001). “How Psychological Science Informs the Teaching of Reading,” Psychological Science in the Public Interest 2/2: 31–74.Google ScholarPubMed
Rhodes, Susan. (1997). “The Active and Passive Voice Are Equally Comprehensible in Scientific Writing,” PhD thesis, University of Washington, 1997. Available as of 2014 at https://digital.lib.washington.edu/researchworks/bitstream/handle/1773/9033/9819294.pdf?sequence = 1&isAllowed = y.
Richards, I. A.(1930). Practical Criticism: A Study of Literary Judgment. London: Kegan Paul Trench Trubner.Google Scholar
Ritchey, Kristin A. (2011). “How Generalization Inferences Are Constructed in Expository Text Comprehension,” Contemporary Educational Psychology 36/4: 280–288.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, David. (1975). “Notes on a Schema for Stories,” in Bobrow, David G. and Collins, Alan (eds.), Representation and Understanding. New York: Academic Press: 211–235.Google Scholar
Salager-Meyer, Françoise. (1994). “Hedges and Textual Communicative Function in Medical English Written Discourse,” English for Specific Purposes 13/2: 149–170.Google Scholar
Samuels, S. Jay and Kamil, Michael L.. (1984). “Models of the Reading Process,” Handbook of Reading Research 1: 185–224.Google Scholar
Schank, Roger C. (1982). Dynamic Memory: A Theory of Reminding and Learning in Computers and People. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, Roger and Abelson, Robert. (1977). Scripts, Plans, Goals and Understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Schnotz, Wolfgang and Kürschner, Christian. (2007). “A Reconsideration of Cognitive Load Theory,” Educational Psychology Review 19: 469–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwinges, Rainer Christoph. (1996). “Student Education, Student Life,” in de Ridder-Symoens, Hilde (ed.), Universities in Early Modern Europe, 1500–1800. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press: 195–243.Google Scholar
Shanahan, Timothy. (1984). “Nature of the Reading–Writing Relation: An Exploratory Multivariate Analysis,” Journal of Educational Psychology 76/3: 466–477.Google Scholar
Shanahan, Timothy. (2006). “Relations among Oral Language, Reading, and Writing Development,” in MacArthur, C. A., Graham, S., and Fitzgerald, J. (eds.), Handbook of Writing Research. New York: Guildford Press: 171–186.Google Scholar
Shanahan, Timothy and Lomax, Richard G.. (1986). “An Analysis and Comparison of Theoretical Models of the Reading–Writing Relationship,” Journal of Educational Psychology 78/2: 116–123.Google Scholar
Sloan, Gary. (1988). “Relational Ambiguity between Sentences,” College Composition and Communication 39/2: 154–165.Google Scholar
Smith, Frank. (2012). Understanding Reading: A Psycholinguistic Analysis of Reading and Learning to Read. 6th edn. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Speedie, Lynn J., Wertman, Eliahu, Ta'ir, Judy, and Heilman, Kenneth M.. (1993). “Disruption of Automatic Speech Following a Right Basal Ganglia Lesion,” Neurology 43/9: 1768–1774.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Wilson, Deirdre. (1985). “Loose Talk,” Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society 86/1: 153–171.Google Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Wilson, Deirdre. (2002). “Pragmatics, Modularity and Mind‐Reading,” Mind & Language 17/1–2: 3–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, Dan and Wilson, Deirdre. (2004). “Relevance Theory,” in Horn, Laurence and Ward, Gregory (eds.), Handbook of Pragmatics. Oxford: Blackwell: 607–632.Google Scholar
Spiro, Rand J. (1980). “Prior Knowledge and Story Processing: Integration, Selection, and Variation,” Poetics 9/1: 313–327.Google Scholar
Spyridakis, Jan H. and Fukuoka, Waka. (2002). “The Effect of Inductively versus Deductively Organized Text on American and Japanese Readers,” IEEE Transactions on Professional Communication 45/2: 99–114.Google Scholar
Stallings, Lynne M., MacDonald, Maryellen C., and O'Seaghdha, Padraig. (1998). “Phrasal Ordering Constraints in Sentence Production: Phrase Length and Verb Disposition in Heavy-NP Shift,” Journal of Memory and Language 39: 392–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinman, Lawrence. (2003). “Collateral Damage Repaired,” Nature 422: 671–672.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stelzner, Hermann G. (1966). “‘War Message,’ December 8, 1941: An Approach to Language,” Communications Monographs 33/4: 419–437.Google Scholar
Temperly, David. (2007). “Minimization of Dependency Length in Written English,” Cognition 105: 300–333.Google Scholar
Thapar, Anjal and Greene, Robert L.. (1993). “Evidence against a Short-term Store Account of Long-Term Recency Effects,” Memory & Cognition 21/3: 329–337.Google Scholar
Therriault, David J. and Raney, Gary E.. (2002). “The Representation and Comprehension of Place-on-the-Page and Text-Sequence Memory,” Scientific Studies of Reading 6/2: 117–134.Google Scholar
Trabasso, Tom and den Broek, Paul van. (1985). “Causal Thinking and the Representation of Narrative Events,” Journal of Memory and Language 24/5: 612–630.Google Scholar
Tversky, Amos and Kahneman, Daniel. (1973). “Availability: A Heuristic for Judging Frequency and Probability,” Cognitive Psychology 5: 207–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Broek, Paul, Lorch, Robert F., Linderholm, Tracy, and Gustafson, Mary. (2001). “The Effects of Readers’ Goals on Inference Generation and Memory for Texts,” Memory & Cognition 29/8: 1081–1087.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, Teun A. (1988). The News as Discourse. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Van Dijk, Teun A. (2008). “Critical Discourse Analysis and Nominalization: Problem or Pseudo-Problem?Discourse & Society 19/6: 821–828.Google Scholar
Wikborg, Eleanor. (1985). “Unspecified Topic in University Student Essays,” Text – Interdisciplinary Journal for the Study of Discourse 5/4: 359–370.Google Scholar
Williams, Joanna P., Taylor, Maravene B., and Ganger, Sonia. (1981). “Text Variations at the Level of the Individual Sentence and the Comprehension of Simple Expository Paragraphs,” Journal of Educational Psychology 73/6: 851–865.Google Scholar
Williams, Joseph M. (1995). Style: Toward Clarity and Grace. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wydick, Richard C. (1998). Plain English for Lawyers. 4th edn. Durham, NC: Carolina Academic Press.Google Scholar
Yore, Larry D. and Shymansky, James A.. (1985). “Reading, Understanding, Remembering and Using Information in Written Science Materials,” in Association for the Education of Teachers in Science. Cincinnati, OH: ERIC: 1–59.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. (1994). “Effect of Genre Expectations on Text Comprehension,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20/4: 920–933.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. (1996). “Processing Narrative Time Shifts,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22/5: 1196–1207.Google Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A., Langston, Mark C., and Graesser, Arthur. (1995). “The Construction of Situation Models in Narrative Comprehension: An Event-Indexing Model,” Psychological Science 6: 292–297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A. and Radvansky, Gabriel A.. (1998). “Situation Models in Language Comprehension and Memory,” Psychological Bulletin 123/2: 162–185.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×