Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T19:16:04.216Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Political Plasticity and Revolution

The Case of Iran

from Part II - Evolution and Involution in Social Transformations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 April 2018

Brady Wagoner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Fathali M. Moghaddam
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
Jaan Valsiner
Affiliation:
Aalborg University, Denmark
Get access

Summary

Why did Iran go from dictatorship to revolution to dictatorship? This question is at the heart of not only the 1979 revolution in Iran, but also most other revolutions – from Russia in 1917 to the recent ‘Arab Spring’. Although psychological research has neglected macro-level change, neuroscientists have studied brain plasticity. The concept of ‘political plasticity’ is used to explore the failure of revolutions to bring about democratic change, with Iran as the case study. Seven rules that regulate political plasticity are introduced: (1) change processes are of two basic types, within and between systems (2) the characteristics of collective processes are more than the sum of smaller units (3) smaller units can only bring about change by acting as part of a larger collective (4) change at macro levels does not necessarily bring about change at micro levels (5) lack of change at the micro level can limit or even reverse change at the macro level (6) political plasticity is regulated by embeddedness, the depth of connections holding a bit of cognition or action in place (7) the depth of embeddedness is dependent on the resiliency and quantity of connectivity. Low political plasticity in areas such as leader-follower relations was a major factor preventing change toward democracy in Iran. Fundamentalist Muslims intent on replacing the dictatorship of the Shah with the dictatorship of the mullahs took advantage of this situation to gain monopoly power.
Type
Chapter
Information
The Psychology of Radical Social Change
From Rage to Revolution
, pp. 122 - 139
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, P. W. (1972). More is different: Broken symmetry and the nature of the hierarchical structure of science. Science, 177, 393396.Google Scholar
Baroncelli, L., Braschi, C., Spolidodo, M., Benegesic, T., Sale, A., and Maffei, L. (2010). Nurturing brain plasticity: Impact of environmental enrichment. Cell Death and Differentiation, 17, 10921103.Google Scholar
Buchan, J. (2013). Days of God: The Revolution in Iran and Its Consequences. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1916/2012). Democracy and Education. New York: Simon and Brown.Google Scholar
Fischer, M. M. J. (1980). Iran: From Religious Dispute to Revolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Chiu, C.-Y., and Hong, Y. Y. (Eds.) (2014). Advances in Culture and Psychology. Vol. 4. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gormley, W. T., and Balla, S. J. (2013). Bureaucracy and Democracy: Accountability and Performance. 3rd ed. Los Angeles, CA.: Sage.Google Scholar
Green, C. S., and Bavelier, D. (2010). Exercising your brain: A review of human brain plasticity and training-induced learning. Psychology and Aging, 23, 692701.Google Scholar
Griebel, U., Pepperberg, I. M., and Oller, D. K. (2016). Developmental plasticity and language: A comparative perspective. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8, 435445.Google Scholar
Harré, R., and Moghaddam, F. M. (Eds.) (2012). Psychology for the Third Millennium. Santa Barbara, CA.: Sage.Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowler, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., et al. (2005). “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 795855.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (2012). The Effects of Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Khomeini, R. (1960). Islamic Governance. Privately published.Google Scholar
Kim, J. (2006). Emergence: Core ideas and issues. Synthese, 151, 547559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., and Gray, R. D. (2012). Tools from evolutionary biology shed new light on the diversification of languages. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 167173.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2002). The Individual and Society: A Cultural Integration. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2005). Great Ideas in Psychology. Oxford: Oneworld.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2008). Multiculturalism and Intergroup Relations: Psychological Implications for Democracy in Global Context. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2013). The Psychology of Dictatorship. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M. (2016). The Psychology of Democracy. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.Google Scholar
Moghaddam, F. M., and Lee, N. (2006). Double reification: The process of universalizing psychology in the Three Worlds. In Brock, A. (Ed.), Internationalizing the History of Psychology (pp. 163182). New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Noddings, N. (2013). Education and Democracy in the 21st Century. New York: Columbia University.Google Scholar
Pahlavi, M. R. (1960). Mission for My Country. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. M., and de la Sablonnière, R. (2014). Toward Constructive Change in Aboriginal Communities. Montreal, Canada: McGill-Queens University Press.Google Scholar
Thomas, C., and Baker, C. I. (2013). Teaching an adult brain new tricks: A critical review of evidence for training-dependent structural plasticity in humans. NeuroImage, 73, 225236.Google Scholar
Voss, M. W., Vivar, C., Kramer, A. F., and Van Praag, H. (2013). Bridging animal and human models of exercise-induced brain plasticity. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 525544.Google Scholar
Watzlawick, P., Weakland, J. H., and Fisch, R. (1974). Change: Principles of Problem Formation and Problem Resolution. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×