Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T16:12:20.185Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7c - Spina bifida/myelomeningocele and hydrocephalus across the lifespan: a developmental synthesis

from Section II - Disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 May 2010

Jacobus Donders
Affiliation:
Mary Free Bed Rehabilitation Hospital
Scott J. Hunter
Affiliation:
University of Chicago
Get access

Summary

Introduction

As has been well outlined in the two chapters regarding spina bifida/myelomeningocele (SB), SB is a still quite common and often severely disabling birth defect that is typically associated with bowel and bladder complications, complete paralysis, and other congenital defects [1], as well as varying degrees of anomalies in the brain. SB affects many aspects of early and later development, impacting opportunities for academic achievement and vocational success, as well as ongoing independence. It is often associated with neurodevelopmental changes, such as Chiari II malformation or acqueductal stenosis, that can result in hydrocephalus [2–5], leading to the need for shunting. Medical management of the conditions associated with SB (e.g. shunting, catheterization) influence the developmental process of cognitive and psychological functioning into adolescence and adulthood.

As both chapters discussed, SB is associated with significant variability in medical, motor, cognitive, psychosocial, academic, and functional adaptation throughout the lifespan. Complications of SB can range from minor physical problems to severe physical and mental disabilities. Generally, the degree and severity of the primary central nervous system (CNS) insult and its potential secondary CNS insults (e.g. hydrocephalus) lead directly to the set of deficits, such as motor functioning and attention orienting [6], that can be seen as early as infancy. This chapter aims to synthesize information shared in the previous two chapters, by reviewing the core deficits and strengths across the lifespan for individuals with SB, and to highlight issues pertinent to assessment, diagnosis, and intervention.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Charney, EB. Neural tube defects: Spina bifida and myelomeningocele. In Batshaw, ML, Perret, YM, eds. Children with Disabilities: A Medical Primer. Baltimore: Brookes; 1992: 471–88.Google Scholar
Fletcher, JM, Copeland, K, Frederick, JA, et al. Spinal lesion level in spina bifida: A source of neural and cognitive heterogeneity. J Neurosurg 2005;102:268–79.Google ScholarPubMed
Juranek, J, Fletcher, JM, Hasan, KM, et al. Neocortical reorganization in spina bifida. Neuroimage 2008;40:1516–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Bigio, MR. Neuropathological changes caused by hydrocephalus. Acta Neuropathol (Berl) 1993;85:573–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Bigio, MR. Cellular damage and prevention in childhood hydrocephalus. Brain Pathol 2004;14:317–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahone, EM, Zabel, TA, Levey, E, et al. Parent and self-report ratings of executive function in adolescents with myelomeningocele and hydrocephalus. Child Neuropsychol 2002;8:258–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, JM, Brookshire, BL, Landry, SH, et al. Attentional skills and executive functions in children with early hydrocephalus. Dev Neuropsychol 1996;12:53–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, HB, Landry, SH, Cohen, L, et al. Early information processing among infants with spina bifida. Presented at the 34th annual meeting of International Neuropsychological Society inBoston, MA, 2006.Google Scholar
Dennis, M, Landry, SH, Barnes, M, et al. A model of neurocognitive function in spina bifida over the life span. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2006;12:285–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, JM, Bohan, TP, Brandt, ME, et al. Morphometric evaluation of the hydrocephalic brain: relationship with cognitive development. Childs Nerv Syst 1996;12:192–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M, Edelstein, K, Frederik, J, et al. Peripersonal spatial attention in children with spina bifida: Associations between horizontal and vertical line bisection and congenital malformations of the corpus callosum, midbrain, and posterior cortex. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:2000–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, JH. Executive processes for children with spina bifida. Child Health Care 1999;28:241–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarazi, R, Mahone, EM, Zabel, TA. Self-care independence in children with neurological disorders: An interactional model of adaptive demands and executive dysfunction. Rehabilitation Psychology 2007;52:196–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahone, EM, Slomine, BS. Managing dysexecutive disorders. In Hunter, SJ, Donders, J, eds. Pediatric Neuropsychological Intervention. New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007: 287–313.Google Scholar
Dennis, M, Jacennik, B, Barnes, MA. The content of narrative discourse in children and adolescents after early-onset hydrocephalus and in normally developing age peers. Brain Lang 1994;46:129–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M, Hendrick, EB, Hoffman, HJ, et al. The language of hydrocephalic children. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1987;9:593–621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, CM. Rehabilitation of children with spina dysraphism. Neurosurg Clin N Am 1995;6:393.Google Scholar
Miall, RC, Reckess, GZ, Imamizu, H. The cerebellum coordinates eye and hand tracking movements. Nat Neurosci 2001;4:638–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, SH, Lomax-Bream, L, Barnes, M. The importance of early motor and visual functioning for later cognitive skills in preschoolers with and without spina bifida. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2003;9:176.Google Scholar
Verhoef, M, Barf, HA, Post, MW, et al. Functional independence among young adults with spina bifida, in relation to hydrocephalus and level of lesion. Dev Med Child Neurol 2006;48:114–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lennerstrand, G, Gallo, JE, Samuelsson, L. Neuro–ophthalmological findings in relation to CNS lesion in patients with myelomeningocele. Dev Med Child Neurol 1990;32:423–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M, Fletcher, JM, Rogers, T, et al. Object-based and action-based visual perception in children with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:95–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rourke, BP. The syndrome of nonverbal learning disabilities: developmental manifestations in neurological disease, disorder, and dysfunction. Clinical Neuropsychol 1988;2:293–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommet, C, Billard, C, Gillet, P, et al. Neuropsychologic and adaptive functioning in adolescents and young adults shunted for congenital hydrocephalus. J Child Neurol 1999;14:144–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennis, M, Rogers, T, Barnes, MA. Children with spina bifida perceive visual illusions but not multistable figures. Brain Cogn 2002;46:108–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, KO, Enrile, BG. Implicit and explicit memory in children with congenital and acquired brain disorder. Neuropsychology 2005;19:618–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, MA, Fletcher, JM, Brookshire, BL, et al. Memory functions in children with early hydrocephalus. Neuropsychology 1998;12:578–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, KO, Enrile, BG, Loss, N, et al. Verbal learning and memory in children with myelomeningocele. J Pediatric Psychol 1995;20:801–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, M, Jewell, D, Drake, J, et al. Prospective, declarative, and nondeclarative memory in young adults with spina bifida. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007;13:312–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barf, HA, Verhoef, M, Jennekens-Schinkel, A, et al. Cognitive status of young adults with spina bifida. Dev Med Child Neurol 2003;45:813–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, MA, Wilkinson, M, Boudousquie, A, et al. Arithmetic processing in children with spina bifida: calculation accuracy, strategy use, and fact retrieval fluency. J Learn Disabil 2006;39:174–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, MA, Dennis, M, Hetherington, R. Reading and writing skills in young adults with spina bifida and hydrocephalus. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10:680–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemanek, KL, Jones, ML, Lieberman, B. Mothers of children with spina bifida: adaptational and stress processing. Children's Health Care 2000;29:19–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coakley, RM, Holmbeck, GN, Bryant, FB. Constructing a prospective model of psychosocial adaptation in young adolescents with spina bifida: an application of optimal data analysis. J Pediatr Psychol 2006;31:1084–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C, Kogan, BA, Parekh, A. Impact of urinary incontinence on self-concept in children with spina bifida. J Urol 2004;171:1659–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, JM, Brookshire, BL, Landry, SH, et al. Behavioral adjustment of children with hydrocephalus: relationships with etiology, neurological, and family status. J Pediatr Psychol 1995;20:109–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmbeck, GN, Westhoven, VC, Shapera, WE, et al. A multi-method, multi-informant, and multi-dimensional perspective on psychosocial adjustment in pre-adolescents with spina bifida. J Consult Clin Psychol 2003;71:782–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmbeck, GN, Shapera, WE. Research methods with adolescents. In Kendall, PC, Butcher, JN, Holmbeck, GN, eds. Handbook of Research Methods in Clinical Psychology, 2nd Edn. New York: Wiley; 1999: 634–61.Google Scholar
Blum, R. Chronic illness and disability in adolescence. J Adolesc Health 1992;13:364–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolman, C, Basco, . Factors influencing self-esteem and self-consciousness in adolescents with spina bifida. J Adolesc Health 1994;15:543–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nassau, JH, Drotar, D. Social competence among children with central nervous system-related chronic health conditions: a review. J Pediatr Psychol 1997;22:771–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, D, Holmbeck, GN, DeLucia, C, et al. Trajectories of autonomy development across the adolescent transition in children with spina bifida. Rehabil Psychol 2009;54:16–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawin, KJ, Brei, TJ, Buran, CF. Factors associated with quality of life in adolescents with spina bifida. J Holist Nurs 2002;20:279–304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holmbeck, GN, Gorey-Ferguson, L, Hudson, T, et al. Maternal, paternal, and marital functioning in families of preadolescents with spina bifida. J Pediatr Psychol 1997;22:167–81.Google ScholarPubMed
Landry, SH, Smith, KE, Miller-Loncar, CL, et al. Predicting cognitive-linguistic and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Dev Psychol 1997;33:1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lomax-Bream, L, Taylor, HB, Landry, SH, et al. Role of early parenting and motor skills on development in children with spina bifida. J Appl Dev Psychol 2007;28(3):250–63.
Vermaes, I, Gerris, J, Jansses, J. Parents ' social adjustment in families of children with spina bifida: A theory-driven review. J Pediatr Psychol 2007;32:1214–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenley, RN, Holmbeck, GN, Rose, BM. Predictors of parenting behavior trajectories among families of young adolescents with and without spina bifida. J Pediatric Psychol 2006;31:1057–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, D, Holmbeck, GN, Jandasek, B, et al. Parent functioning in families of preadolescents with spina bifida: longitudinal implications for child adjustment. J Fam Psychol 2004;18:609–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loomis, JW, Javornisky, JG, Monahan, JJ, et al. Relations between family environment and adjustment outcomes in young adults with spina bifida. Dev Med Child Neurol 1997;39:620–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Natriello, G, McDill, EL, Pallas, AM. Schooling Disadvantaged Children: Racing Against Catastrophe. New York: Teachers College Press; 1990.Google Scholar
Holmbeck, GN, Coakley, RM, Hommeyer, JS, et al. Observed and perceived dyadic and systemic functioning in families of preadolescents with spina bifida. J Pediatr Psychol 2002;27:177–89.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buran, CF, McDaniel, AM, Brei, TJ. Needs assessment in a spina bifida program: a comparison of the perceptions by adolescents with spina bifida and their parents. Clin Nurse Spec 2002;16:256–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, S, O'Connell, B, Pearce, J. The transition from paediatric to adult health care services for children with acquired or congenital disorders. Aust Health Rev 2003;26:64–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, MM, Blackorby, J. Transition from high school to work or college: how special education students fare. Future Child 1996;6:103–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, KL, Dudgeon, B, Kuehn, C, et al. Assistive technology use among adolescents and young adults with spina bifida. Am J Public Health 2007;97:330–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, MA, Smith-Chant, B, Landry, SH. Number processing in neurodevelopmental disorders: spina bifida myelomeningocele. In Campbell, JID, ed. Handbook of Mathematical Cognition. New York: Psychology Press; 2005: 299–314.Google Scholar
Landry, SH, Smith, KE, Swank, PR, et al. Responsive parenting: the optimal timing of an intervention across early childhood. Dev Psychol in press.
Salmon, K. Commentary. Preparing children for medical procedures: taking account of memory. J Pediatr Psychol 2006;31:859–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacy, M, Pyykkonen, BA, Hunter, SJ, et al. Intellectual functioning in children with early-shunted post-hemorrhagic hydrocephalus. Pediatric Neurosurg 2008;44:376–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×