Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T04:08:42.390Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Metacognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2022

Bennett L. Schwartz
Affiliation:
Florida International University
Michael J. Beran
Affiliation:
Georgia State University
Get access

Summary

Metacognition, or awareness of one’s cognition, involves several different but overlapping cognitive abilities, such as working memory, explicit memory, monitoring, and control. These processes are guided by multiple internal or external signals, including memory signals in the case of metamemory. Primate species including apes and rhesus monkeys have demonstrated that they can respond to both internal and external signals, and like humans, these signals can be additive and fallible. In the past few decades, there have been about five dozen studies published on nonhuman animal metacognition and while robust results have been obtained, rigorous experimental paradigms have been employed, and general progress has been made, there is a still a lot we do not know. For example, there are only a few species whose metacognitive abilities have been relatively well characterized, and even in those species significant open questions remain. After an introduction, what is known and what is not known will be explored. Similarities and differences among different primate species will be highlighted. As the chapter is comparative in nature, disparities in behavioral findings across apes and monkeys, New World and Old World monkeys, as well as primates and non-primate species will be explored. The extent to which methods can or cannot be standardized across species will be discussed, with special consideration of species’ ecological niches and experimental methods typically employed. Limitations in nonhuman metacognition research will also be considered, including the fact that most metacognition studies focus on just one species. Finally, possibilities for promising future directions in research will be offered.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, A., & Santi, A. (2011). Pigeons exhibit higher accuracy for chosen memory tests than for forced memory tests in duration matching-to-sample. Learning & Behavior, 39, 111.Google Scholar
Anderson, C. J., Bahnik, S., Barnett-Cowan, M., Bosco, F. A., Chandler, J., Chartier, C. R., Cheung, F., Christopherson, C. D., Cordes, A., Cremata, E. J., Della Penna, N., Estel, V., Fedor, A., Fitneva, S. A., Frank, M. C., Grange, J. A., Hartshorne, J. K., Hasselman, F., Henninger, F., van der Hulst, M., Jonas, K. J., Lai, C. K., Levitan, C. A., Miller, J. K., Moore, K. S., Meixner, J. M., Munafo, M. R., Neijenhuijs, K. I., Nilsonne, G., Nosek, B. A., Plessow, F., Prenoveau, J. M., Ricker, A. A., Schmidt, K., Spies, J. R., Stieger, S., Strohminger, N., Sullivan, G. B., van Aert, R. C. M., van Assen, M., Vanpaemel, W., Vianello, M., Voracek, M., & Zuni, K. (2016, March). Response to Comment on “Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science”. Science, 351(6277).Google Scholar
Armitage, K. L., Bulley, A., & Redshaw, J. (2020). Developmental origins of cognitive offloading. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 287, 9, Article 20192927.Google Scholar
Basile, B. M., Hampton, R. R., Suomi, S., & Murray, E. A. (2008). An assessment of memory awareness in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 12(169180.Google Scholar
Basile, B. M., Schroeder, G. R., Brown, E. K., Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2015). Evaluation of seven hypotheses for metamemory performance in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 144, 85102.Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1950). The Snark was a Boojum. American Psychologist, 5, 115124.Google Scholar
Belger, J., & Brauer, J. (2018). Metacognition in dogs: Do dogs know they could be wrong? Learning & Behavior, 46, 398413.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beran, M. (2012). Did you ever hear the one about the horse that could count? Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 357.Google Scholar
Beran, M. (2019). Animal metacognition: A decade of progress, problems, and the development of new prospects. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 223229.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Brandl, J., Perner, J., & Proust, J. (2012). Foundations of metacognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Menzel, C. R., Parrish, A. E., Perdue, B. M., Sayers, K., Smith, J. D., & Washburn, D. A. (2016). Primate cognition: Attention, episodic memory, prospective memory, self-control, and metacognition as examples of cognitive control in nonhuman primates. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science, 7, 294316.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Church, B. A., & Smith, J. D. (2016). Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modulate their use of an uncertainty response depending on risk. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 42, 3243.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., Futch, S. E., Smith, J. D., Evans, T. A., & Parrish, A. E. (2015). Go when you know: Chimpanzees’ confidence movements reflect their responses in a computerized memory task. Cognition, 142, 236246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beran, M. J., Perdue, B. M., & Smith, J. D. (2014). What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 40, 303316.Google ScholarPubMed
Beran, M. J., & Smith, J. D. (2011). Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Cognition, 120, 90105.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., Coutinho, M. V. C., Couchman, J. J., & Boomer, J. (2009). The psychological organization of “uncertainty” responses and “middle” responses: A dissociation in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 371381.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., & Perdue, B. M. (2013). Language-trained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) name what they have seen but look first at what they have not seen. Psychological Science, 24, 660666.Google Scholar
Beran, M. J., Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., & Washburn, D. A. (2006). Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) monitor uncertainty during numerosity judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 111119.Google Scholar
Beran, M. M., & Beran, M. J. (2004). The roots of human behavior are found in nonhuman primates. American Journal of Primatology, 63, 3335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, R. J., & Hampton, R. R. (2021). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) monitor evolving decisions to control adaptive information seeking. Animal Cognition, 24, 777785.Google Scholar
Brauer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Visual perceptive taking in dogs (Canis familiaris) in the presence of barriers. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 88, 299317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, E. K., Basile, B. M., Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2019). Dissociation of memory signals for metamemory in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 22, 331341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, E. K., Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2017). An assessment of domain-general metacognitive responding in rhesus monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 135, 132144.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caletti, E., Pagliari, C., Vai, B., Delvecchio, G., & Brambilla, P. (2020). Which are the best questionnaires to longitudinally evaluate mindfulness skills in personality disorders? Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 169174.Google Scholar
Call, J. (2010). Do apes know that they could be wrong? Animal Cognition, 13, 689700.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Carpenter, M. (2001). Do apes and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition, 4, 207220.Google Scholar
Carruthers, P., & Williams, D. (2019). Comparative metacognition. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 278288.Google Scholar
Castro, L., & Wasserman, E. A. (2013). Information-seeking behavior: Exploring metacognitive control in pigeons. Animal Cognition, 16, 241254.Google Scholar
Comstock, G. (2019). What do we need to know to know that animals are conscious of what they know? Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 289308.Google Scholar
Crystal, J. D. (2014). Where is the skepticism in animal metacognition? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 152154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewsbury, D. A. (1978). Comparative animal behavior. McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Eaton, T., Billette, P., & Vonk, J. (2020). Are there metacognitivists in the fox hole? A preliminary test of information seeking in an arctic fox (Vulpes lagopus). Behavioral Sciences, 10, article 81.Google Scholar
Faith, L. A., Lecomte, T., Corbiere, M., Francoeur, A., Hache-Labelle, C., & Lysaker, P. H. (2020). Metacognition is uniquely related to concurrent and prospective assessments of negative symptoms independent of verbal memory in serious mental illness. Journal of Nervous Mental Disease, 208, 837842.Google Scholar
Ferrigno, S., Bueno, G., & Cantlon, J. (2019). A similar basis for judging confidence in monkeys and humans. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 335343.Google Scholar
Ferrigno, S., Kornell, N., & Cantlon, J. F. (2017). A metacognitive illusion in monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 284, 6, article 20171541.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906911.Google Scholar
Fleming, S. M., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2012). Metacognition: Computation, biology and function Introduction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences, 367, 12801286.Google Scholar
Foote, A. L., & Crystal, J. D. (2007). Metacognition in the rat. Current Biology, 17, 551555.Google Scholar
Foote, A. L., & Crystal, J. D. (2012). “Play it Again”: A new method for testing metacognition in animals. Animal Cognition, 15, 187199.Google Scholar
Fujita, K. (2009). Metamemory in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Animal Cognition, 12, 575585.Google Scholar
Garcia-Mieres, H., Lundin, N. B., Minor, K. S., Dimaggio, G., Popolo, R., Cheli, S., & Lysaker, P. H. (2020). A cognitive model of diminished expression in schizophrenia: The interface of metacognition, cognitive symptoms and language disturbances. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 131, 169176.Google Scholar
Garland, T. (1994). Quantitative genetics of locomotor behavior and physiology in a garter snake. In Boake, C. R. B. (Ed.), Quantitative genetic studies of behavioural evolution (pp. 251277). The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. A., Rogers, J., Katze, M. G., Bumgarner, R., Weinstock, G. M., Mardis, E. R., Remington, K. A., Strausberg, R. L., Venter, J. C., Wilson, R. K., Batzer, M. A., Bustamante, C. D., Eichler, E. E., Hahn, M. W., Hardison, R. C., Makova, K. D., Miller, W., Milosavljevic, A., Palermo, R. E., Siepel, A., Sikela, J. M., Attaway, T., Bell, S., Bernard, K. E., Buhay, C. J., Chandrabose, M. N., Dao, M., Davis, C., Delehaunty, K. D., Ding, Y., Dinh, H. H., Dugan-Rocha, S., Fulton, L. A., Gabisi, R. A., Garner, T. T., Godfrey, J., Hawes, A. C., Hernandez, J., Hines, S., Holder, M., Hume, J., Jhangiani, S. N., Joshi, V., Khan, Z. M., Kirkness, E. F., Cree, A., Fowler, R. G., Lee, S., Lewis, L. R., Li, Z. W., Liu, Y. S., Moore, S. M., Muzny, D., Nazareth, L. V., Ngo, D. N., Okwuonu, G. O., Pai, G., Parker, D., Paul, H. A., Pfannkoch, C., Pohl, C. S., Rogers, Y. H., Ruiz, S. J., Sabo, A., Santibanez, J., Schneider, B. W., Smith, S. M., Sodergren, E., Svatek, A. F., Utterback, T. R., Vattathil, S., Warren, W., White, C. S., Chinwalla, A. T., Feng, Y., Halpern, A. L., Hillier, L. W., Huang, X. Q., Minx, P., Nelson, J. O., Pepin, K. H., Qin, X., Sutton, G. G., Venter, E., Walenz, B. P., Wallis, J. W., Worley, K. C., Yang, S. P., Jones, S. M., Marra, M. A., Rocchi, M., Schein, J. E., Baertsch, R., Clarke, L., Csuros, M., Glasscock, J., Harris, R. A., Haviak, P., Jackson, A. R., Jiang, H. Y., Liu, Y., Messina, D. N., Shen, Y. F., Song, H. X. Z., Wylie, T., Zhang, L., Birney, E., Han, K., Konkel, M. K., Lee, J. N., Smit, A. F. A., Ullmer, B., Wang, H., Xing, J., Burhans, R., Cheng, Z., Karro, J. E., Ma, J., Raney, B., She, X. W., Cox, M. J., Demuth, J. P., Dumas, L. J., Han, S. G., Hopkins, J., Karimpour-Fard, A., Kim, Y. H., Pollack, J. R., Vinar, T., Addo-Quaye, C., Degenhardt, J., Denby, A., Hubisz, M. J., Indap, A., Kosiol, C., Lahn, B. T., Lawson, H. A., Marklein, A., Nielsen, R., Vallender, E. J., Clark, A. G., Ferguson, B., Hernandez, R. D., Hirani, K., Kehrer-Sawatzki, H., Kolb, J., Patil, S., Pu, L. L., Ren, Y., Smith, D. G., Wheeler, D. A., Schenck, I., Ball, E. V., Chen, R., Cooper, D. N., Giardine, B., Hsu, F., Kent, W. J., Lesk, A., Nelson, D. L., O’Brien, W. E., Prufer, K., Stenson, P. D., Wallace, J. C., Ke, H., Liu, X. M., Wang, P., Xiang, A. P., Yang, F., Barber, G. P., Haussler, D., Karolchik, D., Kern, A. D., Kuhn, R. M., Smith, K. E., Zwieg, A. S., & Rhesus Macaque Genome Sequencing, C. (2007). Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science, 316, 222234.Google Scholar
Goto, K., & Watanabe, S. (2012). Large-billed crows (Corvus macrorhynchos) have retrospective but not prospective metamemory. Animal Cognition, 15, 2735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampton, R. R. (2001). Rhesus monkeys know when they remember. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98, 53595362.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R. (2009). Multiple demonstrations of metacognition in nonhumans: Converging evidence or multiple mechanisms? Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 4, 1728.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R. (2019). Monkey metacognition could generate more insight. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 230235.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R., Engelberg, J. W. M., & Brady, R. J. (2020). Explicit memory and cognition in monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 138, article 107326.Google Scholar
Hampton, R. R., Zivin, A., & Murray, E. A. (2004). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) discriminate between knowing and not knowing and collect information as needed before acting. Animal Cognition, 7, 239254.Google Scholar
Inman, A., & Shettleworth, S. J. (1999). Detecting metamemory in nonverbal subjects: A test with pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 25, 389395.Google Scholar
Iwasaki, S., Kuroshima, H., & Fujita, K. (2019). Pigeons show metamemory by requesting reduced working memory loads. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 247253.Google Scholar
Iwasaki, S., Watanabe, S., & Fujita, K. (2013). Do pigeons (Columba livia) seek information when they have insufficient knowledge? Animal Cognition, 16, 211221.Google Scholar
Iwasaki, S., Watanabe, S., & Fujita, K. (2018). Pigeons (Columba livia) know when they will need hints: Prospective metacognition for reference memory? Animal Cognition, 21, 207217.Google Scholar
James, B. T., Parrish, A. E., Guild, A. S., Creamer, C., Kelly, V., Perdue, B., Kelly, A. J., & Beran, M. J. (2021). Go if you know: Preschool children’s movements reflect their metacognitive monitoring. Cognitive Development, 57, 101001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jozefowiez, K., Staddon, J. E. R., & Cerutti, D. T. (2009). Metacognition in animals: How do we know that they know? Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 4.Google Scholar
Kirk, C. R., McMillan, N., & Roberts, W. A. (2014). Rats respond for information: Metacognition in a rodent? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 40, 249259.Google Scholar
Koriat, A. (2018). When reality is out of focus: Can people tell whether their beliefs and judgments are correct or wrong? Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 147, 613631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Adiv, S., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2014). The effects of goal-driven and data-driven regulation on metacognitive monitoring during learning: A developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 143, 386403.Google Scholar
Kornell, N. (2009). Metacognition in humans and animals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 1115.Google Scholar
Kornell, N. (2014). Where is the “meta” in animal metacognition? Journal of Comparative Psychology, 128, 143149.Google Scholar
Kornell, N., Son, L. K., & Terrace, H. S. (2007). Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychological Science, 18, 6471.Google Scholar
Le Pelley, M. E. (2012). Metacognitive monkeys or associative animals? Simple reinforcement learning explains uncertainty in nonhuman animals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 38, 686708.Google Scholar
Lee, K. A., Preston, A. J., Wise, T. B., & Templer, V. L. (2018). Testing for metacognitive responding using an odor-based delayed match-to-sample test in rats. Jove-Journal of Visualized Experiments, article e57489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malassis, R., Gheusi, G., & Fagot, J. (2015). Assessment of metacognitive monitoring and control in baboons (Papio papio). Animal Cognition, 18, 13471362.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. L. (2014). Metacognitive-like information seeking in lion-tailed macaques: A generalized search response after all? Animal Cognition, 17, 13131328.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. (2019). The information-seeking paradigm: Moving beyond “if and when” to “what, where, and how”. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 329334.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. L., & MacDonald, S. E. (2012a). Information seeking by orangutans: A generalized search strategy? Animal Cognition, 15, 293304.Google Scholar
Marsh, H. L., & MacDonald, S. E. (2012b). Orangutans (Pongo abelii) “play the odds”: Information-seeking strategies in relation to cost, risk, and benefit. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 263278.Google Scholar
Martin-Ordas, G., Berntsen, D., & Call, J. (2013). Memory for distant past events in chimpanzees and orangutans. Current Biology, 23, 14381441.Google Scholar
McMahon, S., Macpherson, K., & Roberts, W. A. (2010). Dogs choose a human informant: Metacognition in canines. Behavioural Processes, 85, 293298.Google Scholar
Nakamura, N., Watanabe, S., Betsuyaku, T., & Fujita, K. (2011). Do birds (pigeons and bantams) know how confident they are of their perceptual decisions? Animal Cognition, 14, 8393.Google Scholar
Nelson, T. O. (1996). Consciousness and metacognition. American Psychologist, 51, 102116.Google Scholar
Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125322.Google Scholar
Nordahl, H., & Wells, A. (2019). Predictors of work ability in individuals with a common mental disorder: Is there an effect of metacognitive beliefs among poor physical health and emotional distress? Behaviour Change, 36, 252262.Google Scholar
Papageorgiou, C., & Wells, A. (2003). An empirical test of a clinical metacognitive model of rumination and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 261273.Google Scholar
Paukner, A., Anderson, J. R., & Fujita, K. (2006). Redundant food searches by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apelia): A failure of metacognition? Animal Cognition, 9, 110117.Google Scholar
Perdue, B. M., Church, B. A., Smith, J. D., & Beran, M. J. (2015). Exploring potential mechanisms underlying the lack of uncertainty monitoring in capuchin monkeys. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 28.Google Scholar
Perdue, B. M., Evans, T. A., & Beran, M. J. (2018). Chimpanzees show some evidence of selectively acquiring information by using tools, making inferences, and evaluating possible outcomes. Plos One, 13, e0193229.Google Scholar
Perry, C. J., & Barron, A. B. (2013). Honey bees selectively avoid difficult choices. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 1915519159.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., & Suddendorf, T. (2020). Temporal junctures in the mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 5264.Google Scholar
Roberts, W. A., Feeney, M. C., McMillan, N., MacPherson, K., Musolino, E., & Petter, M. (2009). Do pigeons (Columba livia) study for a test? Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 35, 129142.Google Scholar
Rosati, A. G., & Santos, L. R. (2016). Spontaneous metacognition in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 27, 11811191.Google Scholar
Royka, A. L., Johnston, A. M., & Santos, L. R. (2020, August). Metacognition in canids: A comparison of dogs (Canis familiaris) and dingoes (Canis dingo). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 134, 303317.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. (2019). Using natural ecology to predict higher cognition in human and non-human primates. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 344354.Google Scholar
Schwartz, B. L., Benjamin, A. S., & Bjork, R. A. (1997). The inferential and experiential bases of metamemory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 6, 132137.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010a). Clever animals and killjoy explanations in comparative psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 477481.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2010b). Cognition, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J., & Sutton, J. E. (2003). Animal metacognition? It’s all in the methods. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26, 353.Google Scholar
Shields, W. E., Smith, J. D., & Washburn, D. A. (1997). Uncertain responses by humans and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) in a psychophysical same-different task. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 126, 147164.Google Scholar
Smith, J., Jackson, B., & Church, B. (2019). The cognitive architecture of uncertainty. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 236246.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D. (2009). The study of animal metacognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 389396.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Beran, M. J., Couchman, J. J., & Coutinho, M. V. C. (2008). The comparative study of metacognition: Sharper paradigms, safer inferences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 679691.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Boomer, J., Church, B. A., Zakrzewski, A. C., Beran, M. J., & Baum, M. L. (2018). I scan, therefore I decline: The time course of difficulty monitoring in humans (Homo sapiens) and macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132, 152165.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Coutinho, M. V. C., Church, B. A., & Beran, M. J. (2013). Executive-attentional uncertainty responses by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 142, 458475.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., Beran, M. J., & Washburn, D. A. (2006). Dissociating uncertainty responses and reinforcement signals in the comparative study of uncertainty monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 135, 282297.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Redford, J. S., Beran, M. J., & Washburn, D. A. (2010). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) adaptively monitor uncertainty while multi-tasking. Animal Cognition, 13, 93101.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Schull, J., Strote, J., McGee, K., Egnor, R., & Erb, L. (1995a). The uncertain response in the bottle-nosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 124, 391408.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Schull, J., Strote, J., McGee, K., Egnor, R., & Erb, L. (1995b). The uncertain response in the bottlenosed dolphin (Tursiops truncatus). Journal of Experimental Psychology-General, 124, 391408.Google Scholar
Smith, J. D., Shields, W. E., Schull, J., & Washburn, D. A. (1997). The uncertain response in humans and animals. Cognition, 62, 7597.Google Scholar
Smith, T. R., Parrish, A. E., Creamer, C., Rossettie, M., & Beran, M. J. (2020). Capuchin monkeys (sometimes) go when they know: Confidence movements in Sapajus apella. Cognition, 199, Article 104237.Google Scholar
Smith, T. R., Smith, D. J., & Beran, M. J. (2018). Not knowing what one knows: A meaningful failure of metacognition in capuchin monkeys. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 5, 5567.Google Scholar
Sole, L. M., Shettleworth, S. J., & Bennett, P. J. (2003). Uncertainty in pigeons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 738745.Google Scholar
Son, L. K., & Kornell, N. (2018). Research on the allocation of study time: Key studies from 1890 to the present (and beyond). In Dunlosky, J. & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.), A handbook of memory and metamemory (pp. 333351). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Suda-King, C. (2008). Do orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) know when they do not remember? Animal Cognition, 11, 2142.Google Scholar
Suda-King, C., Bania, A. E., Stromberg, E. E., & Subiaul, F. (2013). Gorillas’ use of the escape response in object choice memory tests. Animal Cognition, 16, 6584.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. E., & Shettleworth, S. J. (2008). Memory without awareness: Pigeons do not show metamemory in delayed matching to sample. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 34, 266282.Google Scholar
Takagi, S., & Fujita, K. (2018). Do capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) know the contents of memory traces? A study of metamemory for compound stimuli. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 132, 8896.Google Scholar
Taylor, H. K., Cunningham, C. L., & Hardie, S. (2020). Do lemurs know when they could be wrong? An investigation of information seeking in three species of lemur (Lemur catta, Eulemur rubriventer, and Varecia variegata). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 134, 241251.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L. (2019a). Tools of engagement: Information seeking in chimpanzees. Learning & Behavior, 47, 107108.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L. (2019b). Slow progress with the most widely used animal model: Ten years of metacognition research in rats, 2009–2019. Animal Behavior and Cognition, 6, 273277.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L., Brown, E. K., & Hampton, R. R. (2018). Rhesus monkeys metacognitively monitor memories of the order of events. Scientific Reports, 8, Article 11541.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L., & Hampton, R. R. (2012). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) show robust evidence for memory awareness across multiple generalization tests. Animal Cognition, 15, 409419.Google Scholar
Templer, V. L., Lee, K. A., & Preston, A. J. (2017). Rats know when they remember: Transfer of metacognitive responding across odor-based delayed match-to-sample tests. Animal Cognition, 20, 891906.Google Scholar
Tu, H. W., Pani, A. A., & Hampton, R. R. (2015). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) adaptively adjust information seeking in response to information accumulated. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 129, 347355.Google Scholar
Vining, A. Q., & Marsh, H. L. (2015). Information seeking in capuchins (Cebus apella): A rudimentary form of metacognition? Animal Cognition, 18, 667681.Google Scholar
Washburn, D. A., Smith, J. D., & Shields, W. E. (2006). Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) immediately generalize the uncertain response. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 32, 185189.Google Scholar
Watanabe, A., & Clayton, N. S. (2016). Hint-seeking behaviour of western scrub-jays in a metacognition task. Animal Cognition, 19, 5364.Google Scholar
Wells, A. (1995). Meta-cognition and worry: A cognitive model of generalized anxiety dsorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 23, 301320.Google Scholar
Yuki, S., & Okanoya, K. (2017, January). Rats show adaptive choice in a metacognitive task with high uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition, 43, 109118.Google Scholar
Zentall, T., & Wasserman, E. A. (Eds.) (2012). The Oxford handbook of comparative cognition. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zentall, T. R., & Stagner, J. P. (2010). Pigeons prefer conditional stimuli over their absence: A comment on Roberts et al. (2009). Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 506509.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×