Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:37:46.450Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Phonological Transfer in Heritage Japanese in Australia

L1 and L2 Comparisons, and Literacy and Community Contributions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2024

Rajiv Rao
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Madison
Get access

Summary

This study addresses the following questions: (1) What kind of unique phonological characteristics do heritage Japanese learners share? (2) Does L1 or L2 Japanese share these characteristics? (3) Do these characteristics correspond in oral and written production? (4) What could affect phonological transfer? Longitudinal/cross-sectional data to identify phonological transfer from English to heritage Japanese were collected from diary entries/compositions as well as oral and written tests by heritage Japanese learners growing up in Australia, compositions and written tests by Japanese monolinguals in Japan, and essay translations by L1 English speakers learning Japanese as an L2 in Australia. Possible phonological transfer features in heritage Japanese were examined to see if they occur in the L1 and L2. Correspondences between oral and written transfer features were also investigated, and quantitative analyses examined factors associated with phonological transfer. Findings indicate that phonological transfer is acquisitional, possibly influenced by literacy, the mental lexicon, and both sociocultural and individual factors.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chi, M. T. H. (2009). Active-Constructive-Interactive: A conceptual framework for differentiating learning activities. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 73105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clyne, M., Fernandez, S., Chen, I. M., & Summo-O’Connell, R. (1997). Background speakers: Diversity and its management in LOTE programs. Language Australia.Google Scholar
De Houwer, A. (1990). The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, A., & Nakamura, J. (2022). Developmental perspectives on parents’ use of discourse strategies with bilingual children. In Blackwood, R. & Røyneland, U. (Eds.), Multilingualism across the lifespan (pp. 3155). Routledge.Google Scholar
Döpke, S. (1992). One parent one language: An interactional approach. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Döpke, S., McNamara, T. F., & Quinn, T. J. (1991). Psycholinguistic aspects of bilingualism. In Liddicoat, A. J. (Ed.), Bilingualism and bilingual education (pp. 2181). National Languages Institute of Australia.Google Scholar
Forrin, N. D., & MacLeod, C. M. (2018). This time it’s personal: The memory benefit of hearing oneself. Memory, 26(4), 574579.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldstein, B. E. (2018). Cognitive psychology: Connecting mind, research, and everyday experience (5th ed.). Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Hanafusa, K. (2020). The development of reading and writing abilities among Japanese children: A 3-year follow-up study from preschool to the second grade. Journal of Okayama Medical Society, 132, August, 7482.Google Scholar
Hirata, Y. (2015). L2 phonetics and phonology. In Kubozono, H. (Ed.), Handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology (pp. 719762). De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Roodenrys, S., Brown, G., & Mercer, R. (1995). The role of long-term memory mechanisms in memory span. British Journal of Psychology, 86(4), 527536.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ijuin, M., & Wydell, T. (2018). A reading model from the perspective of Japanese orthography: Connectionist approach to the hypothesis of granularity and transparency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 490498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inagaki, K., Hatano, G., & Otake, T. (2000). The effect of kana literacy acquisition on the speech segmentation unit used by Japanese young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 75(1), 7091.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Irwin, M. (2016). The morphology of English loanwords. In Kageyama, T. & Kishimoto, H. (Eds.), Handbook of Japanese lexicon and word formation (pp. 161198). De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarema, G., & Libben, G. (2007). Introduction: Matters of definition and core perspectives. In Jarema, G. & Libben, G. (Eds.), The mental lexicon: Core perspectives (pp. 16). Brill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubozono, H. (2015a). Introduction to Japanese phonetics and phonology. In Kubozono, H. (Ed.), Handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology (pp. 142). De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kubozono, H. (2015b). Loanword phonology. In Kubozono, H. (Ed.), Handbook of Japanese phonetics and phonology (pp. 313362). De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindholm, K., & Padilla, A. (1978). Language mixing in bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 5(2), 327335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Llombart-Huesca, A. (2018). Understanding the spelling errors of Spanish heritage language learners. Hispania, 101(2), 211223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minagawa, Y., Maekawa, K., & Kiritani, S. (2002). Effects of pitch accent and syllable position in identifying Japanese long and short vowels: Comparison of English and Korean speakers. Journal of the Phonetic Society of Japan, 6(2), 8897.Google Scholar
Mizutani, N. (1994). Jitsurei de manabu goyō bunseki no hōhō. [The error analysis methodology with actual examples.] Aruku.Google Scholar
Nagaoka, Y. (1998). A descriptive study of Japanese biliterate students in the United States: Bilingualism, language-minority education, and teachers’ role [Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts Amherst].Google Scholar
Odlin, T. (1989). Language transfer: Cross-linguistic influence in language learning (Cambridge Applied Linguistics). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ota, M. (2013). Lexical frequency effects on phonological development: The case of word production in Japanese. In Vihman, M. & Keren-Portnoy, T. (Eds.), The emergence of phonology: Whole-word approaches and cross-linguistic evidence (pp. 415438). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pienemann, M. (1998) Language processing and second language development: Processability theory. John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rao, R. (2016). Introduction to special issue on heritage phonetics and phonology. Heritage Language Journal, 13(2), iii.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saunders, G. (1991). Sociolinguistic aspects of bilingualism: Bilingualism and society. In Liddicoat, A. J. (Ed.), Bilingualism and bilingual education (pp. 81141). National Languages Institute of Australia.Google Scholar
Seymour, P. H. K. (1992). Cognitive theories of spelling and implications for education. In Sterling, C. M. & Robson, C. (Eds.), Psychology, spelling and education (pp. 5070). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Shinnouchi, M. (2000). Gendai gairaigo jijoo [The contemporary loanwords situation]. Data and Statistics, 510, 813.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Blackwell.Google ScholarPubMed
Uno, A., Wydell, T. N., Haruhara, N., Kaneko, M., & Shinya, N. (2009). Relationship between reading/writing skills and cognitive abilities among Japanese primary-school children: Normal readers versus poor readers (dyslexics). Reading & Writing, 22, 755789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×