Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T09:20:51.994Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Childhood Language Exposure

Does Early Experience with Arabic Affect Sound Perception and Production in Speakers with Early Interrupted Exposure?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 February 2024

Rajiv Rao
Affiliation:
University of Wisconsin, Madison
Get access

Summary

Early language exposure is crucial for acquiring native mastery of phonology, and multilingual exposure results in enhanced phonetic/phonological learning ability in adulthood. It remains unclear, however, whether early language exposure has lasting benefits when the quantity and quality of speaking drop dramatically after childhood. We investigate the production and perception of Arabic in fifteen early-interrupted exposure (i.e., childhood) speakers and fifteen late-exposure (i.e., novice) speakers. We compare the production of both groups to that of a control group of fifteen early uninterrupted exposure (i.e., native monolingual) speakers. The experiment included tasks addressing language proficiency, word production, and speech perception. Early-interrupted exposure speakers outperformed late-exposure speakers on all tasks of the language proficiency diagnostic, while also displaying more native-like perception and production. Our study adds further support to the body of work on the measurable long-term benefits of early language experience for an individual’s phonetic and phonological skills, even when language experience diminishes over time.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abu-Abbas, K. H., Zuraiq, W. M., & Abdel-Ghafer, O. A. (2011). Geminates and long consonants in Jordanian Arabic. International Journal of Linguistics, 3(1). https://doi.org/10.5296/ijl.v3i1.735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Pasquale, A., Green, D., Hernandez, M., Scifo, P., Keim, R., Cappa, S., & Costa, A. (2011). Bilingualism tunes the anterior cingulate cortex for conflict monitoring. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 20762086. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Albirini, A., Benmamoun, E., & Saadah, E. (2011). Grammatical features of Egyptian and Palestinian Arabic heritage speakers’ oral productions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(2), 273303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alkhudidi, A., Rafat, Y., & Stevenson, R. (2020). Geminate attrition in the speech of Arabic–English bilinguals living in Canada. Heritage Language Journal, 17(1), 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al-Sobh, M., Abu-Melhim, A., & Bani-Hani, N. (2015). Diglossia as a result of language variation in Arabic: Possible solutions in light of language planning. Journal of Language Teaching and Research, 6(2), 274279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Al Suwaiyan, L. A. (2018). Diglossia in the Arabic language. International Journal of Language and Linguistics, 5(3), 228238.Google Scholar
Al-Tamimi, F., Abu-Abbas, K., & Tarawneh, R. (2010). Jordanian Arabic final geminates: An experimental clinical phonetic study. Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 46(2), 111125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anthony, J., & Francis, D. (2005). Development of phonological awareness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5), 255259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archila-Suerte, P., Zevin, J., Bunta, F., Hernandez, A. E. (2011). Age of acquisition and proficiency in a second language independently influences the perception of non-native speech. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(1), 190201. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influence of bilingualism on cognitive strategy and cognitive development. Child Development, 48, 10091018.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Best, C., & McRoberts, G. W. (2003). Infant perception of non-native consonant contrasts that adults assimilate in different ways. Language and Speech, 46(2–3), 183216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2018). Bilingualism and executive function: What’s the connection? In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields (pp. 283305). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.54.13bia.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bongaerts, T., Planken, B., & Schils, E. (1995). Can late starters attain a native accent in a foreign language? A test of the critical period hypothesis. In Singleton, D. & Lengyel, Z. (Eds.), The age factor in second language acquisition (pp. 3050). Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Burns, T., & Werker, J. (2010). The roots of bilingualism in newborns. Psychological Science, 21(3), 343348. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609360758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, K., Deng, Y., Li, M., & Yan, H. M. (2015). The impact of L2 learning on cognitive aging. ADMET & DMPK, 3(3), 260273. https://doi.org/10.5599/admet.3.3.206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciccone, M., Hanini, R., & Sciannantena, M. (2020). A cross-linguistic examination of geminate consonant attrition. In Pavlova, A. (Ed.), Proceedings of the ESSLLI & WeSSLLI Student Session 2020 (pp. 173184). Brandeis University.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. (Ed.). (2009). The world’s major languages (2nd ed.). Taylor & Francis. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203301524.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1994). Developmental differences in infant attention to the spectral properties of infant-directed speech. Child Development, 65(6), 1663. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I. Y., Katz, L., & Tola, G. (1988). Awareness of phonological segments and reading ability in Italian children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Research, 49(2), 222251. https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543049002222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Del Maschio, N., & Abutalebi, J. (2018). Neurobiology of bilingualism. In Miller, D., Bayram, F., Rothman, J., & Serratrice, L. (Eds.), Bilingual cognition and language: The state of the science across its subfields (pp. 325346). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demont, E., & Gombert, J. E. (1996). Phonological awareness as a predictor of recoding skills and syntactic awareness as a predictor of comprehension skills. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 315332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2000). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of Arabic-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 451471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. (1987). A critical period for learning to pronounce foreign languages? Applied Linguistics, 8, 162177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flege, J. E., & Fletcher, K. L. (1992). Talker and listener effects on degree of perceived foreign accent. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 91(1), 370389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fromkin, V. A., Krashen, S., Curtiss, S., Rigler, D., & Rigler, M. (1974). The development of language in Genie: A case of language acquisition beyond the “critical period.” Brain and Language, 1, 81107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillon, G. T. (2017). Phonological awareness: From research to practice (2nd ed.). Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(2), 387396. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3837-12.2013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H. (2011). Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49, 38263830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. W. (2011). Bilingual worlds. In Cook, V. & Bassetti, B. (Eds.), Language and bilingual cognition (pp. 243254). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hernandez, A. E., & Li, P. (2007). Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin, 133(4), 638650. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 12291243. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2000). Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 131138. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364–6613(00)01463-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, S., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Left hemisphere cerebral specialization for babies while babbling. Science, 297(5586), 1515. https://doi.org/10.1126/Science.1074941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holowka, S., Brosseau-Lapré, F., & Petitto, L. A. (2002). Semantic and conceptual knowledge underlying bilingual babies’ first signs and words. Language Learning, 52, 205262. https://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 228. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2002.2670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansen, W. (2004). Laryngeal contrast and phonetic voicing: A laboratory phonology approach to English, Hungarian, and Dutch [Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen, Netherlands].Google Scholar
Jasińska, K. K., & Petitto, L.-A. (2017). Age of bilingual exposure is related to the contribution of phonological and semantic knowledge to successful reading development. Child Development, 89(1), 310311. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12745.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Friederici, A. D., Wessels, J. M., Svenkerud, V. Y., & Jusczyk, A. M. (1993). Infants’ sensitivity to the sound patterns of native language words. Journal of Memory and Language, 32(3), 402420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klimova, B., Valis, M., & Kuca, K. (2017). Bilingualism as a strategy to delay the onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Clinical Interventions in Aging, 12, 17311737. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S145397.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovelman, I., Baker, S., & Petitto, L.A. (2008). Age of first bilingual language exposure as a new window into bilingual reading development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11, 203223. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831843. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., Tsao, F. M., & Liu, H. M. (2003). Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(15) 90969101. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1532872100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). The biological foundations of language. Hospital Practice, 2(12), 5967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levi, S. V., Winters, S. J., & Pisoni, D. B. (2007). Speaker-independent factors affecting the perception of foreign accent in a second language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(4), 23272338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, M. A. (1998). Language acquisition and the Conrad phenomenon. IRAL, International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 36(1), 6981.Google Scholar
Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(46), 1680816813. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4563-11.2011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Major, R. C. (2010). First language attrition in foreign accent perception. International Journal of Bilingualism, 14(2), 163183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marzecova, A. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive control: A Loch Ness Monster case or an instance of neural plasticity? Cortex, 73, 364366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masgoret, A. M., & Gardner, R. C. (2003). Attitudes, motivation, and second language learning: A meta-analysis of studies conducted by Gardner and associates. Language Learning, 53, 167210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.2.179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. (2004). Children’s literacy development. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, T. (1990). Pronouncing Arabic 1. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moyer, A. (1999). Ultimate attainment in L2 phonology. The critical factors of age, motivation and instruction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 81108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Beyond phonological skills: Broader language skills contribute to the development of reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 342356. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00238.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, J. S., Jun, S. A., Knightly, L. M., & Au, T. K. F. (2003). Holding on to childhood language memory. Cognition, 86(3), B53B64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pa, J., & Hickok, G. (2008). A parietal-temporal sensory-motor integration area for the human vocal tract: Evidence from an fMRI study of skilled musicians. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 362368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, J. W., & MacAskill, M. R. (2018). Building experiments in PsychoPy. Sage Publishing.Google Scholar
Peirce, J. W., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M. R., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, E., & Lindeløv, J. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods, 51, 195203. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, L.-A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., Dubins, M. H., Jasińska, K., & Shalinsky, M. (2012). The “perceptual wedge hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121, 130143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulin-Dubois, D., Bialystok, E., Blaye, A., Polonia, A., & Yott, J. (2013). Lexical access and vocabulary development in very young bilinguals. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17, 5770. https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911431198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prior, A., & MacWhinney, B. (2010). A bilingual advantage in task switching. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(2), 253262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, K. R., Landi, N., Preston, J. L., Mencl, W. E., Austin, A. C., Sibley, D., & Frost, S. J. (2013). The relationship between phonological and auditory processing and brain organization in beginning readers. Brain and Language, 125, 173183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.04.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rafat, Y., Mohaghegh, M., & Stevenson, R. A. (2017). Geminate attrition across three generations of Farsi-English bilinguals living in Canada: An acoustic study. Ilha do Desterro, 70(3), 151168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiterer, S., Hu, X., Erb, M., Rota, G., Nardo, D., Grodd, W., Winkler, S., & Ackermann, H. (2011). Individual differences in audio-vocal speech imitation aptitude in late bilinguals: Functional neuro-imaging and brain morphology. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbah, S. (2015). Is Standard Arabic dying? Arab World English Journal, 6(2), 5465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2019). What is phonological awareness in L2? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 50, 1727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak: A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech. Newbury House Publishers.Google Scholar
Sebastián-Gallés, N., Albareda-Castellot, B., Weikum, W. M., & Werker, J. F. (2012). A bilingual advantage in visual language discrimination in infancy. Psychological Science, 23(9), 994999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Spinu, L. E., Hwang, J., & Lohmann, R. (2018). Is there a bilingual advantage in phonetic and phonological acquisition? The initial learning of word-final coronal stop realization in a novel accent of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 22(3), 350370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spinu, L., Hwang, J., Pincus, N., & Vasilita, M. (2020). Exploring the use of an artificial accent of English to assess phonetic learning in monolingual and bilingual speakers. In Proceedings of Interspeech 2020, 2377–2381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & Jerman, O. (2007). The influence of working memory on reading growth in subgroups of children with reading disabilities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 249283. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2006.12.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, M. C., & Sabourin, L. (2012). Comparing behavioral discrimination and learning abilities in monolinguals, bilinguals and multilinguals. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(5), 34653474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, R .K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192212. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wardhaugh, R., & Fuller, J. (2015). An introduction to sociolinguistics. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Yuan, E., (2011). Zoom Video Communications [Computer program]. https://zoom.us/.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×