Book contents
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of figures
- List of tables
- Contributors
- Part I Introduction
- Part II Frameworks and conceptual issues
- 2 Manifestations of an epistemological belief system in preschool to grade twelve classrooms
- 3 Epistemic climate in elementary classrooms
- 4 The integrative model of personal epistemology development: theoretical underpinnings and implications for education
- 5 An epistemic framework for scientific reasoning in informal contexts
- 6 Who knows what and who can we believe? Epistemological beliefs are beliefs about knowledge (mostly) to be attained from others
- Part III Students' personal epistemology, its development, and its relation to learning
- Part IV Teachers' personal epistemology and its impact on classroom teaching
- Part V Conclusion
- Index
5 - An epistemic framework for scientific reasoning in informal contexts
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 May 2010
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of figures
- List of tables
- Contributors
- Part I Introduction
- Part II Frameworks and conceptual issues
- 2 Manifestations of an epistemological belief system in preschool to grade twelve classrooms
- 3 Epistemic climate in elementary classrooms
- 4 The integrative model of personal epistemology development: theoretical underpinnings and implications for education
- 5 An epistemic framework for scientific reasoning in informal contexts
- 6 Who knows what and who can we believe? Epistemological beliefs are beliefs about knowledge (mostly) to be attained from others
- Part III Students' personal epistemology, its development, and its relation to learning
- Part IV Teachers' personal epistemology and its impact on classroom teaching
- Part V Conclusion
- Index
Summary
Introduction
In the science education community, there is a growing consensus that in addition to conceptual knowledge, we need to introduce learners to another important facet of science, that is, how we create new knowledge. In other words, students should be better educated in the use of certain established ways of thinking in science (e.g., Duschl, 1990; Lawson et al., 2000). The “certain established ways of thinking in science” are commonly referred to as scientific reasoning, which is portrayed by philosophers of science as a process of argumentation (Giere, 1991; Seigel, 1988; Toulmin, 1958), because it involves the evaluation of evidence to support a theory or claim. In schools, scientific reasoning is usually presented in domain-specific contexts such as physics, chemistry, life sciences, and so forth. Nevertheless, as Seigel (1988) has noted, the commitment to evidence is an imperative trait of rational reasoning in many disciplines, although the form it takes may vary with the disciplines. Even in everyday situations, testing of the possibilities with accountable evidence or reasons and searching for possibilities are critical for decision-making (Baron 1988; Kuhn, 1991; Lawson et al., 2000; Perkins and Salman, 1989). Hence, although scientific reasoning is often discussed within specific knowledge domains, as Kuhn (1993) pointed out, it represents a domain-independent mode of argumentative reasoning.
The development of scientific reasoning has been widely discussed in psychological research (Zimmerman, 2000). Nevertheless, most studies are placed in domain-specific contexts and deal with well-structured problems with only a few exceptions (Kuhn, 1991, 1993).
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- Personal Epistemology in the ClassroomTheory, Research, and Implications for Practice, pp. 124 - 162Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 2010
- 9
- Cited by