Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:47:34.560Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Qualitative Research on Children's Peer Relations in Cultural Context

from Part I - Culture and Peer Relationships: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

William A. Corsaro
Affiliation:
Indiana University Bloomington, Indiana, USA
Xinyin Chen
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
Doran C. French
Affiliation:
Illinois Wesleyan University
Barry H. Schneider
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Get access

Summary

Recently, we have seen important changes in the conceptualization of human development in psychology, sociology, and anthropology. In general, these changes involve a greater focus on children's agency, more concern for the importance of social and cultural context, and agreement that children's experiences beyond their early years in the family (especially their interactions and experiences with peers) are in need of more careful theoretical consideration and empirical research. Also, at least in sociology and anthropology, there is recognition that children both affect and are affected by society and culture. This recognition has increased the appreciation of the creativity and autonomy of children's peer cultures and the awareness that the nature and quality of children's lives, even in their first years, is enriched or constrained by power relations and social policies in the cultures in which they live (Corsaro & Fingerson, 2003).

Although some aspects of children's peer relations and cultures may be universal, the way these aspects are developed and manifested, and other features of children's cultures, are clearly affected by the larger cultural context in which they develop (Corsaro, 1988, 1994, 2005; Corsaro, Molinari, & Rosier, 2002). Therefore, in studying children's peer relations, it is necessary to take a comparative perspective and to attempt to identify how key processes in children's peer cultures both affect and are affected by the larger cultural structures and processes within which they are embedded.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrahams, R. (1975). Negotiating respect: Patterns of presentation among Black women. In Farrer, C. R. (Ed.), Women and folklore (pp. 58–80). Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Adler, P. A., & Adler, P. (1998). Peer power: Preadolescent culture and identity. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Alderson, P. (2000). Children as researchers: The effects of participation rights on research methodology. In Christensen, P. & James, A. (Eds.), Research with children: Perspectives and practices (pp. 241–257). London: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
Aydt, H., & Corsaro, W. A. (2003). Differences in children's construction of gender across culture: An interpretive approach. American Behavioral Scientist, 46, 1306–1325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berentzen, S. (1984). Children constructing their social world. University of Bergen: Bergen, Norway.Google Scholar
Best, R. (1983). We've all got scars: What boys and girls learn in elementary school. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Christensen, P. & James. A. (2000a). Introduction: Researching children and childhood: Cultures of communication. In Christensen, P. & James, A. (Eds.), Research with children: Perspectives and practices (pp. 1–8). London: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
Christensen, P. & James, A. (2000b). Childhood diversity and commonality: Some methodological insights. In Christensen, P. & James, A. (Eds), Research with children: Perspectives and practices (pp. 160–178). London: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
Connell, R. (1987). Gender and power: Society, the person and sexual politics. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (1985). Friendship and peer culture in the early years. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (1988). Routines in the peer culture of American and Italian nursery school children. Sociology of Education, 61, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (1992). Interpretive reproduction in children's peer cultures. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55, 160–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (1994). Discussion, debate, and friendship: Peer discourse in nursery schools in the US and Italy. Sociology of Education, 67, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (2003). “We're friends, right?”: Inside kids' culture. Washington, DC: Joseph Henry Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A. (2005). The sociology of childhood (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Eder, D. (1990). Children's peer cultures. Annual Review of Sociology, 16, 197–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Fingerson, L. (2003). Development and socialization in childhood. In Delamater, J. (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 125–155). New York: Kluwer/Plenum.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W., & Molinari, L. (1990). From seggiolini to discussione: The generation and extension of peer culture among Italian preschool children. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 3, 213–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Molinari, L. (2000a). Entering and observing in children's worlds: A reflection on a longitudinal ethnography of early education in Italy. In Christensen, P. & James, A. (Eds.), Research with children: Perspectives and practices (pp. 179–200). London: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Molinari, L. (2000b). Priming events and Italian children's transition from preschool to elementary school: Representations and actions. Social Psychology Quarterly, 63, 16–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Molinari, L. (2005). I compagni: Understanding children's transition from preschool to elementary school. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., Molinari, L., Hadley, K., & Sugioka, H. (2003). Keeping and making friends in Italian children's transition from preschool to elementary school. Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 272–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W., Molinari, L., & Rosier, K. (2002). Zena and Carlotta: Transition narratives and early education in the United States and Italy. Human Development, 45, 323–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Nelson, E. (2003). Children's collective activities and peer culture in early literacy in American and Italian preschools. Sociology of Education, 76, 209–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, B. (1989). Frogs and snails and feminist tales: Preschool children and gender. Boston: Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Eder, D., & Fingerson, L. (2002). Interviewing children and adolescents. In Gubrium, J. F. & Holstein (Eds.), J. A., Handbook of interview research (pp. 181–201). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Evaldsson, A. (1993). Play, disputes and social order: Everyday life in two Swedish after-school centers. Linköping, Sweden: Linköping University.Google Scholar
Evaldsson, A., & Corsaro, W. A. (1998). Play and games in the peer culture of preschool and preadolescent children: An interpretive approach. Childhood, 5, 377–402.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernie, D., Davies, B, Kantor, R. & McMurray, P. (1993). Becoming a person in the preschool: Creating integrated gender, school culture, and peer culture positionings. Qualitative Studies in Education, 6, 95–110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fine, G. (1987). With the boys: Little league baseball and preadolescent culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fine, G., & Sandstrom, K. (1988). Knowing children: Participant observation with minors. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaskins, S., Miller, P. J., & Corsaro. W. A. (1992). Theoretical and methodological perspectives in the interpretive study of children. In Corsaro, W. A. & Miller, P. J. (Eds.), Interpretive approaches to children's socialization, (pp. 5–23). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, L. R. (1998). Child's play: Myth, mimesis and make-believe. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1985). The serious side of jump rope: Conversational practices and social organization in the frame of play. Folklore, 98, 315–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1990). He-said-she-said: Talk as social organization among Black children. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (1998). Games of stance: Conflict and footing in hopscotch. In Hoyle, S. & Adger, C. T. (Eds.), Kids' talk: Strategic language use in later childhood (pp. 23–46). New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Goodwin, M. H. (2003) The relevance of ethnicity, class, and gender in children's peer negotiations. In Holmes, J. & Meyerhoff, M. (Eds.), Handbook of language and gender (pp. 229–251). New York: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadley, K. (2003). Children's word play: Resisting and accommodating Confucian values in a Taiwanese kindergarten classroom. Sociology of Education, 76, 193–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, T. (1998). At home in the street: Street children of northeast Brazil. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holmes, R. (1995). How children perceive race. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
James, A. (2000). Embodied beings: Understanding the self and the body in childhood. In Prout, A. (Ed.), The body, childhood and society (pp. 19–37). New York: St. Martin's Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, A., Jenks, C., & Prout, A. (1998). Theorizing childhood. New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Katriel, T. (1987). “Bexibùdim!”: Ritualized sharing among Israeli children's conflicts. Language in Society, 16, 305–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyratzis, A., & Guo, J. (2001). Preschool girls' and boys' verbal conflict strategies in the United States and China. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 34, 45–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lever, J. (1976). Sex differences in the games children play. Social Problems, 23, 478–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Løkken, G. (2000). The playful quality of the toddling “style.”International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 13, 531–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Lewis, C. (2003). Less day care or different day care?Child Development, 74, 1069–1075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayall, B. (2002). Towards a sociology for childhood: Thinking from children's lives. Philadelphia: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Messner, M. A. (2000). Barbie girls versus sea monsters: Children constructing gender. Gender & Society, 14, 765–784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishler, E. (1979). “Won't you trade cookies with the popcorn?”: The talk of trades among six year olds. In Garnica, O. & King, M. (Eds.), Language, children, and society: The effects of social factors on children's learning to communicate (pp. 21–36). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Mussati, T., & Panni., S. (1981). Social behavior and interaction among day care center toddlers. Early Child Development and Care, 7, 5–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. (2003). Does amount of time spent in child care predict socio-emotional adjustment during the transition to kindergarten?Child Development, 74, 976–1005.CrossRef
Prout, A. (2000). Childhood bodies: Construction, agency and hybridity. In Prout, A. (Ed.), The body, childhood and society, (pp. 1–18). New York: St. Martin's Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzo, T. A. (1989). Friendship development among children in school. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Rizzo, T. A. (1992). The role of conflict in children's friendship development. In Corsaro, W. A. & Miller, P. J. (Eds.), Interpretive approaches to children's socialization (pp. 93–111). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (1997). Pretend play as improvisation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Selman, R. (1980). The growth of personal understanding. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shantz, C. (1987). Conflicts between children. Child Development, 58, 283–305.
Strandell, H. (1997). Doing reality with play: Play as a children's resource in organizing everyday life in daycare centres. Childhood, 4, 445–464.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tesson G., & Youniss, J. (1995). Micro-sociology and psychological development: A sociological interpretation of Piaget's theory. In Ambert, A. (Ed.), Sociological studies of children (vol. 7 pp. 101–126). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Thelen E., & Smith, L. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In Lerner, R. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 563–634). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thorne, B. (1986). Girls and boys together … but mostly apart: Gender arrangements in elementary schools. In Hartrup, W. & Rubin, Z. (Eds.), Relationships and development (pp. 167–184). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Thorne, B. (1993). Gender play: Girls and boys in school. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. V. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Walkerdine, V. (1990). Schoolgirl fictions. New York: Verso Press.Google Scholar
Williams, S. J., & Bendelow, G. A. (1998). Malignant bodies: Children's beliefs about health, cancer and risk. In Nettleton, S. & Watson, J. (Eds.), The body in everyday life. (pp. 103–123). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Winterhoff, P. A. (1997.) Sociocultural promotions constraining children's social activity: Comparisons and variability in the development of friendships. In Tudge, J., Shanahan, M., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.), Comparisons in human development: Understanding time and context (pp. 222–251). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Youniss, J., & Damon, W. (1994). Social construction and Piaget's theory. In Puka, B. (Ed.), Moral Development: Vol. 5, New research in moral development (pp. 407–426). New York: Garland.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×