Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T08:29:15.833Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Newborn behavior and perception

from Section 2 - Sensory systems and behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2011

Hugo Lagercrantz
Affiliation:
Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm
M. A. Hanson
Affiliation:
Southampton General Hospital
Laura R. Ment
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Donald M. Peebles
Affiliation:
University College London
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Traditionally, the behavior of human newborns has been considered to be essentially reflexive over the first few weeks of life. However, more recently evidence has accumulated to indicate that newborns are already well equipped to acquire and learn information rapidly. In addition, through prenatal learning and other mechanisms, newborns are biased to orient and attend to specific types of information that will become of high relevance to their future learning and survival. In this chapter, we begin by outlining evidence for the learning abilities of newborns, before discussing some domains for which there is evidence that newborns have specific biases or abilities.

Two abilities that are already present at birth are orienting to novelty and habituation to repeated stimuli. These processes can be revealed by registering changes in newborns' looking and sucking behavior. Cardiac and brain activity measures, when incorporated in careful experimental design, may also shed light on newborns' brain function.

Orienting

Orienting towards new stimuli is not only essential for the survival of any organism, it is also a key prerequisite of learning. Therefore, it is of primary importance for neonates. The orienting reflex is a combination of overt and covert responses associated with searching for and preferential processing of new information (Pavlov, 1927; Sokolov, 1963; Sokolov et al., 2002).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Newborn Brain
Neuroscience and Clinical Applications
, pp. 185 - 198
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrams, R. M. & Gerhardt, K. J. (2000). The acoustic environment and physiological responses of the fetus. Journal of Perinatology, 20, S31–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, J. (2000). The Developing Visual Brain. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bertoncini, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., Blumstein, S. E., et al. (1987). Discrimination in neonates of very short CVs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 82, 31–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambrige, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bystrova, K., Widstrom, A. M., Matthiesen, A. S., et al. (2003). Skin-to-skin contact may reduce negative consequences of “the stress of being born”: a study on temperature in newborn infants, subjected to different ward routines in St. Petersburg. Acta Paediatrica, 92, 320–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carral, V., Huotilainen, M., Ruusuvirta, T., et al. (2005). A kind of auditory “primitive intelligence” already present at birth. European Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 3201–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., et al. (1998). Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 351–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheour, M., Martynova, O., Naatanen, R., et al. (2002). Speech sounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature, 415, 599–600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarkson, M. G. & Berg, W. K. (1983). Cardiac orienting and vowel discrimination in newborns: crucial stimulus parameters. Child Development, 54, 162–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarkson, M. G., Clifton, R. K., Swain, I. U., et al. (1989). Stimulus duration and repetition rate influence newborn's head orientation toward sound. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 683–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csibra, G., Tucker, L. A., Volein, A., et al. (2000). Cortical development and saccade planning: the ontogeny of the spike potential. Neuroreport, 11, 1069–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csibra, G., Kushnerenko, E., & Grossmann, T. (2008). Electrophysiological methods in studying infant cognitive development. In Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2nd edn., eds. Nelson, C. & Luciana, M.Cambrige, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
DeCasper, A. & Fifer, W. (1980). Of human bonding: newborns prefer their mother's voice. Science, 208, 1174–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 113–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haan, M., Humphries, K., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Developing a brain specialized for face processing: a converging methods approach. Developmental Psychobiology, 40, 200–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene-Lambertz, G. & Pena, M. (2001). Electrophysiological evidence for automatic phonetic processing in neonates. Neuroreport, 12, 3155–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Dehaene, S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2002). Functional neuroimaging of speech perception in infants. Science, 298, 2013–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deregnier, R. A., Nelson, C. A., Thomas, K. M., et al. (2000). Neurophysiologic evaluation of auditory recognition memory in healthy newborn infants and infants of diabetic mothers. Journal of Pediatrics, 137, 777–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., et al. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A, 99, 9602–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., Menon, E., et al. (2005). Newborns' preference for face-relevant stimuli: effects of contrast polarity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A, 102, 17245–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomes, H., Molholm, S., Christodoulou, C., et al. (2000). The development of auditory attention in children. Frontiers in Bioscience, 5, 108–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffiths, S. K., Brown, W. S. Jr., Gerhardt, K. J., et al. (1994). The perception of speech sounds recorded within the uterus of a pregnant sheep. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 96, 2055–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H. (2005a). Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2nd edn. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (2005b). Subcortical face processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 766–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H. & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and Cognitive Development: The Case of Face Recognition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., et al. (1991). Newborns' preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. (2004). Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Stevens, E., Hayashi, A., et al. (2006). Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9, F13–F21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushnerenko, E. (2003). Maturation of the Cortical Auditory Event-Related Brain Potentials in Infancy. Helsinki: University of Helsinki.Google Scholar
Kushnerenko, E. V., Batuev, A. S., & Bystrova, K. S. (1998). Human neonate's reactions to natural olfactory stimuli [abstract]. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 30, 1–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kushnerenko, E. V., Bystrova, K. S., & Batuev, A. S. (1999). The newborn babies' reactions to the olfactory stimuli [in Russian]. Psychologicheski Journal, 4.Google Scholar
Kushnerenko, E., Ceponiene, R., Balan, P., et al. (2002). Maturation of the auditory change-detection response in infants: a longitudinal ERP study. Neuroreport, 13, 1843–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kushnerenko, E., Winkler, I., Horváth, J., et al. (2007). Processing acoustic change and novelty in newborn infants. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 265–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lagercrantz, H. & Slotkin, T. A. (1986). The “stress” of being born. Scientific American, 254, 100–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecanuet, J., Graniere-Deferre, C., Jacquet, A., et al. (2000). Fetal discrimination of low-pitched musical notes. Developmental Psychobiology, 36, 29–31.3.0.CO;2-J>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leppanen, P., Guttorm, T., Pihko, E., et al. (2004). Maturational effects on newborn ERPs measured in the mismatch negativity paradigm. Experimental Neurology, 190 (Suppl. 1), 91–101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewkowicz, D. J. & Turkewitz, G. (1980). Cross-modal equivalence in early infancy: auditory-visual intensity matching. Developmental Psychology, 16, 597–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacFarlane, A. (1975). Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. Ciba Foundation Symposium, 33, 103–17.Google Scholar
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mehler, J., Jusczyk, P., Lambertz, G., et al. (1988). Precursor of language acquisition in young infants. Cognition, 29, 143–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, C. M. & Fifer, W. P. (2000). Evidence of transnatal auditory learning. Journal of Perinatology, 20, S37–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, C., Bever, T. G., & Fifer, W. P. (1992). Canonical and non-canonical syllable discrimination by two-day-old infants. Journal of Child Language, 19, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-old infants prefer their native language. Infant Behavior and Development, 16, 495–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morr, M. L., Shafer, V. L., Kreuzer, J. A., et al. (2002). Maturation of mismatch negativity in typically developing infants and preschool children. Ear and Hearing, 23, 118–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrongiello, B. A. & Clifton, R. K. (1984). Effects of sound frequency on behavioral and cardiac orienting in newborn and five-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 429–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrongiello, B. A., Fenwick, K. D., Hillier, L., et al. (1994). Sound localization in newborn human infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 519–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Näätänen, R., Gaillard, A. W. K., & Mäntysalo, S. (1978). Early selective attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychologica, 42, 313–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Condition Reflexes. New York, NY: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Pena, M., Maki, A., Kovacic, D., et al. (2003). Sounds and silence: an optical topography study of language recognition at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A, 100, 11702–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C., et al. (2000). Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science, 288, 349–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Silva-Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants. Developmental Science, 8, 162–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee, C. K. & Rovee, D. T. (1969). Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 8, 33–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: rethinking the development of implicit and explicit memory. Psychological Review, 104, 467–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruusuvirta, T., Huotilainen, M., Fellman, V., et al. (2003). The newborn human brain binds sound features together. Neuroreport, 14, 2117–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sewards, T. V. & Sewards, M. A. (2002). Innate visual object recognition in vertebrates: some proposed pathways and mechanisms. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology, Part A. Molecular and Integrative Physiology, 132, 861–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simion, F., Valenza, E., Umilta, C., et al. (1998). Preferential orienting to faces in newborns: a temporal-nasal asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance, 24, 1399–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: the orienting reflex. Annual Review of Physiology, 25, 545–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokolov, E. N., Spinks, J. A., Naatanen, R., et al. (2002). The Orienting Response in Information Processing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sullivan, R. M. (2003). Developing a sense of safety: the neurobiology of neonatal attachment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1008, 122–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turati, C., Simion, F., Milani, I., et al. (2002). Newborns' preference for faces: what is crucial?Developmental Psychology, 38, 875–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turkewitz, G., Birch, H. G., & Cooper, K. K. (1972). Responsiveness to simple and complex auditory stimuli in the human newborn. Developmental Psychobiology, 5, 7–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varendi, H. & Porter, R. H. (2001). Breast odour as the only maternal stimulus elicits crawling towards the odour source. Acta Paediatrica, 90, 372–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varendi, H., Porter, R. H., & Winberg, J. (1994). Does the newborn baby find the nipple by smell?Lancet, 344, 989–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varendi, H., Porter, R. H., & Winberg, J. (1996). Attractiveness of amniotic fluid odor: evidence of prenatal olfactory learning?Acta Paediatrica, 85, 1223–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varendi, H., Porter, R. H., & Winberg, J. (2002). The effect of labor on olfactory exposure learning within the first postnatal hour. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 206–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vouloumanos, A. & Werker, J. F. (2007a). Listening to language at birth: evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science, 10, 159–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vouloumanos, A. & Werker, J. F. (2007b). Why voice melody alone cannot explain neonates' preference for speech. Developmental Science, 10, 169–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984a). Cross-language speech perception: evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werker, J. F. & Tees, R. C. (1984b). Phonemic and phonetic factors in adult cross-language speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 75, 1866–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner, L. & Boike, K. (2001). Infants' sensitivity to broadband noise. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109, 2103–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winberg, J. (2005). Mother and newborn baby: mutual regulation of physiology and behavior – a selective review. Developmental Psychobiology, 47, 217–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winkler, I., Kushnerenko, E., Horvath, J., et al. (2003). Newborn infants can organize the auditory world. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the U S A, 100, 11812–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. H. (1966). The causes, controls, and organization of behavior in the neonate. Psychological Issues, 5, 1–105.Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×