Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T02:42:57.001Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 6 - Intelligence as Domain-Specific Superior Reproducible Performance

The Role of Acquired Domain-Specific Mechanisms in Expert Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 January 2018

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux [New methods for the diagnosis of intellectual level of subnormals]. L’Année Psychologique, 11, 191244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1908). Le developpement de l’intelligence chez les enfants [The development of intelligence in children]. L’Année Psychologique, 14, 190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boake, C. (2002). From the Binet-Simon to the Wechsler-Bellevue: Tracing the history of intelligence testing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24, 383405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1981). Skilled memory. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition (pp. 141189). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 158). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Choudhry, N. K., Fletcher, R. H., & Soumerai, S. B. (2005). Systematic review: The relationship between clinical experience and health care. Annals of Internal Medicine, 142, 260273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1976). Approaches to descriptions and analyses of problem-solving processes: The 8-puzzle. Stockholm, Sweden: Department of Psychology, University of Stockholm.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. Academic Medicine, 79, S70–S81.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2006). The influence of experience and deliberate practice on the development of superior expert performance. In Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.). Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance (pp. 685706). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2009). The scientific study of expert levels of performance can guide training for producing superior achievement in creative domains. In: International Conference on the Cultivation and Education of Creativity and Innovation. Beijing, China: Chinese Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2013). Exceptional memory and expert performance: From Simon and Chase’s theory of expertise to skilled memory and beyond. In Staszewski, J. (Ed.), Expertise and skill acquisition (pp. 201228). Abington, Oxon, UK: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2014). Why expert performance is special and cannot be extrapolated from studies of performance in the general population: A response to criticisms. Intelligence, 45, 81103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (in press a). Capturing expert thought with protocol analysis: Concurrent verbalizations of thinking during experts’ performance on representative tasks. In Ericsson, K. A., Hoffman, R. R., Kozbelt, A, & Williams, A. M. (Eds.). 2nd revised edition of Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (in press b). Superior working memory in experts. In Ericsson, K. A., Hoffman, R. R., Kozbelt, A, & Williams, A. M. (Eds.). 2nd revised edition of Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (in press c). The differential influence of experience, practice, and deliberate practice on the development of superior individual performance of experts. In Ericsson, K. A., Hoffman, R. R., Kozbelt, A, & Williams, A. M. (Eds.). 2nd revised edition of Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of a memory skill. Science, 208, 11811182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptations to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Pool, R. (2016). Peak: Secrets from the new science of expertise. New York: Eamon Dolan Books/ Houghton Mifflin & Harcourt.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1993). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (revised edition). Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Smith, J. (1991). Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise: An introduction. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 138). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Ward, P. (2007). Capturing the naturally occurring superior performance of experts in the laboratory: Toward a science of expert and exceptional performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 346350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Whyte, J., & Ward, P. (2007). Expert performance in nursing: Reviewing research on expertise in nursing within the framework of the expert performance approach. Advances in Nursing Science, 30, E58E71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitts, P., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Frey, M. C., & Detterman, D. K. (2004). Scholastic assessment or g? The relationship between the scholastic assessment test and general cognitive ability. Psychological Science, 15(6), 373378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24, 79132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, J. (1887). Experiments on “prehension.Mind, 45, 7579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oxford Dictionary (2016a). Retrieved on November 1, 2016 from https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/intelligence.Google Scholar
Oxford Dictionary (2016b). Retrieved on November 1, 2016 from https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/definition/ability.Google Scholar
Peterson, J. (1925). Early conceptions and tests of intelligence. Yonkers-on-Hudson, NY: World Book.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richman, H. B., Staszewski, J. J., & Simon, H. A. (1995). Simulation of expert memory using EPAM IV. Psychological Review, 102, 305–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruthsatz, J., Detterman, D. K., Griscom, W. S., & Cirullo, B. A. (2008). Becoming an expert in the musical domain: It takes more than just practice. Intelligence, 36, 330338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruthsatz, J., Ruthsatz, K., & Stephens, K. R. (2014). Putting practice into perspective: Child prodigies as evidence of innate talent. Intelligence, 45, 6065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1916). The measurement of intelligence: An explanation of and a complete guide for the use of the Stanford revision and extension of the Binet-Simon intelligence scale. New York: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terman, L. M. (1925). Mental and physical traits of a thousand gifted children. Genetic studies of genius, Vol. 1, Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1939). The measurement of adult intelligence. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yerkes, R. M. (Ed.) (1921). Psychological examining in the United States Army. Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences, 15 (Parts 1–3), Washington, DC: Government Printing Office.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×