Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T21:58:27.667Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Bossing around the forest: power asymmetry and hierarchy

from Part II - How conflicts shape societies

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 May 2016

Ivan Norscia
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi, Pisa
Elisabetta Palagi
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi, Pisa
Get access

Summary

Power must be understood in the first instance as the multiplicity of force relations immanent in the sphere in which they operate and which constitute their own organization: as the process which, through ceaseless struggle and confrontations, transforms, strengthens, or even reverses them; as the support which these force relations find in one another, thus forming a chain or a system, or on the contrary, the disjunctions and contradictions which isolate them from one another; and lastly, as the strategies in which they take effect…Power…is the name that one attributes to a complex strategic situation in a particular society.

Michel Foucault, History of Sexuality, 1976

I've got the power!

A society is not the simple sum of single individuals but an emergent property of their multiple and intertwined relationships, which are able to generate complex relational networks. Dominance relationships within social groups can be based upon the ability of using force, individually or by forming coalitions (intrinsic and derived dominance, respectively; Hand, 1986) or upon leverage (when involving inalienable resources that cannot be taken by force; Lewis, 2002), as extensively explained in Chapter 8.

Over the centuries, power has been one of the main subjects of discussion for philosophers from Niccolò Machiavelli in the sixteenth century (e.g., The Prince) to Michel Foucault in the twentieth century (e.g., History of Sexuality and The Subject and Power), men of letters such as Vittorio Alfieri (e.g., Della Tirannide, nineteenth century), contemporary linguists such as Noam Chomsky (e.g., Understanding Power: The Indispensable, 2002) and scientists such as Frans de Waal (e.g., Chimpanzee Politics: Power and Sex Among Apes, 1982), to cite a few by heart. Social hierarchies, deriving from the different distribution of power among individuals, drive the behaviour in many species, including humans. History is full of examples of the impact that extreme power imbalance can have on societies. The holocaust, the abduction of Sabine women, and the ancient Egyptian pyramidal hierarchy – from farmers and labourers to the pharaoh – are just some of them. In today's society, a corporation is structured as an organisational chart with the Chief Executive Officer (CEO) at the root of the hierarchy.

In humans, social position can affect parental investment, and mental and physical health, for example contributing to hypertension, depression and the increase of stress levels (Adler et al., 2000, 2008).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Missing Lemur Link
An Ancestral Step in the Evolution of Human Behaviour
, pp. 77 - 111
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, E. S. (2005). Bayesian analysis of linear dominance hierarchies. Animal Behaviour, 69, 1191–1201.Google Scholar
Adams, M. J., Majolo, B., Ostner, J., et al. (2015). Personality structure and social style in macaques. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: Personality Processes and Social Psychology, 109, 338–353.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G. & Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychology, 19, 586–592.Google Scholar
Adler, N. E., Singh-Manoux, A., Schwartz, J., et al. (2008). Social status and health: A comparison of British civil servants in Whitehall-II with European- and African-Americans in CARDIA. Social Science and Medicine, 66, 1034–1045.Google Scholar
Alfieri, V. (1996). Della Tirannide, Del Principe e delle Lettere, La Virtù Sconosciuta. Edizione Astense, Rizzoli BUR.
Allee, W. C. (1931). Animal Aggregations. A Study in General Sociology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: sampling methods. Behaviour, 49, 227–266.Google Scholar
Alvarez, F. (1975). Social hierarchy under different criteria in groups of squirrel monkeys, Saimiri sciureus. Primates, 16, 437–455.Google Scholar
Appleby, M. C. (1983). The probability of linearity in hierarchies. Animal Behaviour, 31, 600–608.Google Scholar
Balasubramaniam, K. N., Dittmar, K., Berman, C. M., et al. (2012). Hierarchical steepness and phylogenetic models: phylogenetic signals in Macaca. Animal Behaviour, 83: 1207–1218.Google Scholar
Balasubramaniam, K. N., Berman, C. M., De Marco, A. et al. (2013). Consistency of dominance rank order: A comparison of David's scores with I&SI and Bayesian methods in macaques. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 959–971.Google Scholar
Bearder, S. K. (1987). Lorises, bushbabies, and tarsiers: Diverse societies in solitary foragers. In: Smuts, B. B., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Wrangham, R. W. & Struhsaker, T. T. (eds), Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 11–24.
Bergstrom, M. L. & Fedigan, L. M. (2013). Dominance style of female white-faced capuchins. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 150, 591–601.Google Scholar
Bernstein, I. S. (1981). Dominance: the baby and the bathwater. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 419–457.Google Scholar
Boinski, S. (1999). The social organizations of squirrel monkeys: implications for ecological models of social evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 8, 101–112.Google Scholar
Chance, M. R. A. (1967). Attention structure as the basis of primate rank orders. Man, 2, 503–518.Google Scholar
Charles-Dominique, P. (1977). Ecology and Behaviour of Nocturnal Primates: Prosimians of Equatorial West Africa. London: Duckworth.
Chase, I. D. (1980). Social process and hierarchy formation in small groups: A comparative perspective. American Sociological Review, 45, 905–924.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L. (1977). The acquisition of rank and the development of reciprocal alliances in freeranging immature baboons. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2, 303–318.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., Mitchell, P. R. & Schoeffel, J. (2002). Understanding Power: The Indispensable, 9th edition. The New Press, New York.
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1982). Red Deer. Behaviour and Ecology of Two Sexes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
Clutton-Brock, T. H., Albon, S. D. & Guinness, F. E. (1984). Maternal dominance, breeding success and birth sex ratios in red deer. Nature, 308, 358–360.Google Scholar
de Vries, H. (1995). An improved test of linearity in dominance hierarchies containing unknown or tied relationships. Animal Behaviour, 50, 1375–1389.Google Scholar
de Vries, H. & Appleby, M. C. (2000). Finding an appropriate order for a hierarchy: a comparison of the I & SI and the BBS methods. Animal Behaviour, 59, 239–245.Google Scholar
de Vries, H., Netto, W. J. & Hanegraaf, P. L. H. (1993). MatMan: a program for the analysis of sociometric matrices and behavioural transition matrices. Behaviour, 125, 157–175.Google Scholar
Vries, H de., Stevens, J. M. G. & Vervaecke, H. (2006). Measuring and testing steepness of dominance hierarchies. Animal Behaviour, 71, 585–592.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (1982). Chimpanzee Politics. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press.
de Waal, F. B. M. (1986). The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. Quarterly Review of Biology, 61, 459–479.Google Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. & Luttrell, L. M. (1985). The formal hierarchy of rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): an investigation of bared-teeth display. American Journal Primatology, 9, 73–86.Google Scholar
Digby, L. J. & Kahlenberg, S. M. (2002). Female dominance in blue-eyed black lemurs (Eulemur macaco flavifrons). Primates, 43, 191–199.Google Scholar
Donati, G., Baldi, N., Morelli, V., Ganzhorn, J. U. & Borgognini-Tarli, S. M. (2009). Proximate and ultimate determinants of cathemeral activity in brown lemurs. Animal Behaviour, 77, 317–325.Google Scholar
Drews, C. (1993). The concept and definition of dominance in animal behaviour. Behaviour, 125, 283–313.Google Scholar
Dunham, A. E. (2008). Battle of the sexes: cost asymmetry explains female dominance in lemurs. Animal Behaviour, 76, 1435–1439.Google Scholar
Duplaix, N. (1980). Observations on the ecology and behavior of the giant river otter Pteronura brasiliensis in Suriname. La Terre et la Vie-Revue d'Ecologie Appliquee, 34, 495–620.Google Scholar
East, M. L., Hofer, H. & Wickler, W. (1993). The erect ‘penis’ is a flag of submission in a female-dominated society: greetings in Serengeti spotted hyenas. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 33, 355–370.Google Scholar
Flack, J. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2004). Dominance style, social power, and conflict. In: Thierry, B., Singh, M. & Kaumanns, W. (eds), Macaque Societies: A Model for the Study of Social Organization. Cambridge University Press, pp. 157–185.
Foucault, M. (1982). The subject and power. Critical inquiry, 8, 777–795.Google Scholar
Fox, S. F., Rose, E. & Myers, R. (1981). Dominance and the acquisition of superior home ranges in the lizard Uta stansburiana. Ecology, 62, 888–893.Google Scholar
Fox, S. F., Rose, E. & Myers, R. (1981). Dominance and the acquisition of superior home ranges in the lizard Uta stansburiana. Ecology, 62, 888–893.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (1984). Symmetrical patterns in non-agonistic social interactions found in unprovisioned Japanese macaques. Journal of Ethology, 2, 109–119.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (1985). Inter-male associations in a wild Japanese macaque troop on Yakushima Island, Japan. Primates, 26, 219–237.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (1989). Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. International Journal of Primatology, 10, 173–197.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (1997). Agonistic interactions and matrifocal dominance rank of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba. International Journal of Primatology, 18, 855–875.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (2009). Factors underlying party size differences between chimpanzees and bonobos: a review and hypotheses for future study. Primates, 50, 197–209.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. (2011). Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society. Evolutionary Anthropology, 20(4), 131–142.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T. & Hashimoto, C. (2002). Why female bonobos have a lower copulation rate during estrus than chimpanzees. In: Boesch, C., Hohmann, G. and Marchant, L. F. (eds), Behavioural Diversity in Chimpanzees and Bonobos. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 156–167.
Furuichi, T., Takasaki, H. & Sprague, D. S. (1982). Winter range utilization of a Japanese macaque troop in a snowy habitat. Folia Primatologica, 37, 77–94.Google Scholar
Furuichi, T., Idani, G., Ihobe, , , H., et al. (2012). Long-term studies on wild bonobos at Wamba, Luo Scientific Reserve, D. R. Congo: towards the understanding of female life history in a male-philopatric species. In: Kappeler, P. M. & Watts, D. P., Long-term Field Studies of Primates: Berlin, Heidelberg, Springer-Verlag, pp. 413–433.
Gammel, M. P., De Vries, H., Jennings, D. J., Carlin, C. M. & Hayden, T. J. (2003). David's score: a more appropriate dominance ranking method than Clutton-Brock, et al.'s index. Animal Behaviour, 66, 601–605.Google Scholar
Gerloff, U., Hartung, , Fruth, B., Hohmann, B., , G. & Tautz, D. (1999). Intracommunity relationships, dispersal pattern and paternity success in a wild living community of bonobos (Pan paniscus) determined from DNA analysis of faecal samples. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 266, 1189–1195.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: a critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 205, 581–598.Google Scholar
Graf, R. P. (1981). Some aspects of snowshoe hare behavioural ecology. M.Sc. Thesis, University of British Columbia.
Grüter, C. C. & Zinner, D. (2004). Nested societies. Convergent adaptations of baboons and snub-nosed monkeys?Primate Report, 70, 1–98.Google Scholar
Guhl, A. M., Collias, N. E. & Allee, W. C. (1945). Mating behavior and social hierarchy in small flocks of white leghorns. Physiological Zoology, 18, 365–390.Google Scholar
Gursky, S. L. (1998). The effect of radio transmitter weight on a small nocturnal primate: Data on activity time budgets, prey capture rates, mobility patterns and weight loss. American Journal of Primatology, 46, 145–155.Google Scholar
Hager, R. & Welker, C. (2001). Female dominance in African lorises (Otolemur garnettii). Folia Primatologica, 72, 48–50.Google Scholar
Hall, C. L. & Fedigan, L. M. (1997). Spatial benefits afforded by high rank in white-faced capuchins. Animal Behaviour, 53, 1069–1082.Google Scholar
Hand, J L. (1986). Resolution of social conflicts: dominance, egalitarianism, spheres of dominance, and game theory. Quarterly Review of Biology, 61, 201–220.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Wobber, V. & Wrangham, R. (2012). The self-domestication hypothesis: evolution of bonobo psychology is due to selection against aggression. Animal Behaviour, 83, 573–585.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, C., Furuichi, T. & Takenaka, O. (1996). Matrilineal kin relationship and social behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus): sequencing the D-loop region of mitochondrial DNA. Primates, 37, 305–318.Google Scholar
Hashimoto, C., Tashiro, Y., Hibino, E., et al. (2008). Longitudinal structure of a unit-group of bonobos: Male philopatry and possible fusion of unit-groups. In: T. Furuichi and J. Thompson. The Bonobos: Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. New York, Springer, pp. 107–119.
Hawley, P. H. (1999). The ontogenesis of social dominance: a strategy-based evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 19, 97–132.Google Scholar
Hemelrijk, C. K. (1999). An individual-oriented model on the emergence of despotic and egalitarian societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 266, 361–369.Google Scholar
Hemelrijk, C. K., Wantia, J. & Gygax, L. (2005). The construction of dominance order: comparing performance of five methods using an individual-based model. Behaviour, 142, 1037–1058.Google Scholar
Hewitt, S. E., Macdonald, D. W. & Dugdale, H. L. (2009). Context-dependent linear dominance hierarchies in social groups of European badgers, Meles meles. Animal Behaviour, 77, 161–169.Google Scholar
Idani, G. (1990). Relations between unit-groups of Bonobos at Wamba, Zaire: encounters and temporary fusions. African Study Monographs, 11, 153–186.Google Scholar
Isbell, L. A. & Young, T. P. (1993). Social and ecological influences on activity budgets of vervet monkeys, and their implications for group living. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 32, 377–385.Google Scholar
Jolly, A. (1966). Lemur Behaviour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Jolly, A. (1984). The puzzle of female feeding priority. In: M. Small (ed.), Female Primates: Studies by Women Primatologists. New York: A. R. Liss, pp. 197–215.
Jolly, S. E. & Spurr, E. B. (1996). Effect of ovariectomy on the social status of brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) in captivity. New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 23, 27–31.Google Scholar
Jolly, A., Caless, S., Cavigelli, S., et al. (2000). Infant killing, wounding, and predation in Eulemur and Lemur. International Journal of Primatology, 23, 327–353.Google Scholar
Kano, T. (1992). The Last Ape: Pygmy Chimpanzee Behavior and Ecology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
Kaplan, J. R. (1987). Dominance and affiliation in the Cercopithecini and Papionini: a comparative examination. In: Zucker, E. L. (ed.), Comparative Behavior of African Monkeys, New York: Alan R. Liss, pp. 127–150.
Kappeler, P. M. (1993). Female dominance in primates and other mammals. In: Bateson, P. P. G., Klopfer, P. H. & Thompson, N. S. (eds), Perspectives in Ethology Vol. 10, Behavior and Evolution. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 143–158.
Kappeler, P. M. (1996). Causes and consequences of life-history variation among strepsirrhine primates. American Naturalist, 148, 868–891.Google Scholar
Kappeler, P. M. (1999). Lemur social structure and convergence in primate socioecology. In: Lee, P. C. (ed.), Comparative Primate Socioecology. Cambridge University Press, pp. 273–299.
Kappeler, P. M. (2000). Causes and consequences of unusual sex ratios among lemurs. In: Kappeler, P. M. (ed.), Primate Males – Causes and Consequences of Variation in Group Composition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 55–63.
Kawai, M., Dunbar, R., Ohsawa, H. & Mori, U. (1983). Social organization of gelada baboons: social units and definitions. Primates, 24, 13–24.Google Scholar
Kemp, C. & Tenenbaum, J. B. B. (2008). The discovery of structural form. PNAS, 105, 10687–10692.Google Scholar
Kendall, M. (1962). Rank Correlation Methods. London: Griffin.
King, A. J., Douglas, C. M. S., Huchard, E., Isaac, N. J. B. & Cowlishaw, G. (2008). Dominance and affiliation mediate despotism in a social primate. Current Biology, 18, 1838–1838.Google Scholar
Kitchen, D. M., Cheney, D. L. & Seyfarth, R. M. (2005). Contextual factor mediating contests between male chacma baboons in Botswana: effects of food, friends, and females. International Journal of Primatology, 26, 105–125.Google Scholar
Klass, K. & Cords, M. (2011). Effect of unknown relationships on linearity, steepness and rank ordering of dominance hierarchies: simulation studies based on data from wild monkeys. Behavioural Processes, 88, 168–176.Google Scholar
Koenig, A. (2000). Competitive regimes in forest-dwelling Hanuman langur females (Semnopithecus entellus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 48, 93–109.Google Scholar
Koren, L., Mokady, O. & Geffen, E. (2006). Elevated testosterone levels and social ranks in female rock hyrax. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 470–477.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B. (1987). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Boston: Blackwell.
Kubzdela, K. S., Richard, A. F. & Pereira, M. E. (1992). Social relations in semi-free-ranging sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi coquereli) and the question of female dominance. American Journal of Primatology, 28, 139–145.Google Scholar
Landau, H. G. (1951). On dominance relations and the structure of animal societies: I. Effect of inherent characteristics. Bulletin of Mathematical Biophysics, 13, 1–19.Google Scholar
Lévi-Strauss, C. (1953). Social structure. In: Kroeber, A. L. (ed.), Anthropology Today. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 524–533.
Lewis, R. J. (2002). Beyond dominance: the importance of leverage. Quarterly Review of Biology, 77, 149–64.Google Scholar
Lewis, R. J. & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Bimorphism in male Verreaux's sifaka in the Kirindy forest of Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 159–182.Google Scholar
Machiavelli, N. (1961). The Prince. London: Penguin.
Maiya, A. S. & Berger-Wolf, T. Y. (2011). Benefits of bias: towards better characterisation of network sampling. Proceedings of the 17th ACM SIGKDD international conference on knowledge discovery and data mining, New York: ACM New York, pp. 105–113. ISBN: 978-1-4503-0813-7.
McGrew, W. C. (1972). An Ethological Study of Children's Behavior. New York: Academic Press.
Meyers, D. & Wright, P. C. (1993). Resource tracking: Food availability and Propithecus seasonal reproduction. In: Kappeler, P. & Ganzhorn, J. (eds), Lemur Social Systems and Their Ecological Basis. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 179–192.
Missakian, E. A. (1976). Aggression and dominance relations in peer groups of children 6–45 months of age. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Animal Behavior Society, Boulder, Colorado.
Monamy, V. (1997). Sexual differences in habitat use by Rattus lutreolus (Rodentia: Muridae): the emergence of patterns in native rodent community structure. Australian Mammalogy, 20, 43–48.Google Scholar
Mooring, M. S., Fitzpatrick, T. A., Benjamin, J. E., et al. (2003). Sexual segregation in desert bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis mexicana). Behaviour, 140, 183–207.Google Scholar
Murie, J. O. & Harris, M. A. (1988). Social interactions and dominance relationships between female and male Columbian ground-squirrels. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 66, 1414–1420.Google Scholar
Murray, C. M. (2007). A method for assigning categorical rank in female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) via the frequency of approaches. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 853–864.Google Scholar
Muscatell, K. A., Morelli, S. A., Falk, E. B., et al. (2012). Social status modulates neural activity in the mentalizing network. NeuroImage, 60, 1771–1777.Google Scholar
Neumann, C., Duboscq, J., Dubuc, C., et al. (2011). Assessing dominance hierarchies: validation and advantages of progressive evaluation with Elo-rating. Animal Behaviour, 82, 911–921.Google Scholar
Neumann, C., Agil, M., Widdig, A. & Engelhardt, A. (2013). Personality of wild male crested macaques (Macaca nigra). PLos ONE, 8, e69383. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069383.Google Scholar
Newton-Fisher, N. E. (2004). Hierarchy and social status in Budongo chimpanzees. Primates, 45, 81–87.Google Scholar
Norscia, I. & Borgognini-Tarli, S. M. (2008). Ranging behavior and possible correlates of pair-living in Southeastern Avahis (Madagascar). International Journal of Primatology, 29, 153–171.Google Scholar
Norscia, I. & Palagi, E. (2011). Do brown lemurs reconcile? Not always. Journal of Ethology, 29, 181–185.Google Scholar
Norscia, I. & Palagi, E. (2015). The socio-matrix reloaded: from hierarchy to dominance profile in wild lemurs. PeerJ, 3, e729. http://dx.doi.org/10.7717/peerj.729.Google Scholar
Norscia, I., Antonacci, D. & Palagi, E. (2009). Mating first, mating more: biological market fluctuation in a wild prosimian. PLoS ONE, 4, e4679. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004679.Google Scholar
Norscia, I., Ramanamanjato, J. B. & Ganzhorn, J. U. (2012). Feeding patterns and dietary profile of nocturnal southern woolly lemurs (Avahi meridionalis) in southeast Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 33, 150–167.Google Scholar
Ogola Onyango, P., Gesquiere, L. R., Wango, E. O., Alberts, S. C. & Altmann, J. (2008). Persistence of maternal effects in baboons: Mother's dominance rank at son's conception predicts stress hormone levels in subadult males. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 319–324.Google Scholar
Palagi, E. & Norscia, I. (2011). Scratching around stress: hierarchy and reconciliation make the difference in prosimians. Stress, 14, 93–97.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Chiarugi, E. & Cordoni, G. (2008a). Peaceful post-conflict interactions between aggressors and bystanders in captive lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla). American Journal of Primatology, 70, 949–955.Google Scholar
Palagi, E., Antonacci, D. & Norscia, I. (2008b) Peacemaking on treetops: first evidence of reconciliation from a wild prosimian (Propithecus verreauxi). Animal Behaviour, 76, 737–747.Google Scholar
Palombit, R. A. (1994). Extra-pair copulations in a monogamous ape. Animal Behaviour, 47, 721–723.
Palombit, R. A. (1995). Longitudinal patterns of reproduction in wild female siamang (Hylobates syndactylus) and white-handed gibbons (Hylobates lar). International Journal of Primatology, 16, 739–760.
Paoli, T. & Palagi, E. (2008). What does agonistic dominance imply in bonobos? In: Takeshi, F. and Thompson, J. (eds), Bonobos: Behaviour, Ecology, and Conservation. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 35–54.
Paoli, T., Palagi, E., Tacconi, G. & Tarli, S. B. (2006). Perineal swelling, intermenstrual cycle, and female sexual behavior in bonobos (Pan paniscus). American Journal of Primatology, 68, 333–347.Google Scholar
Parish, A. R. (1994). Sex and food control in the uncommon chimpanzee: how bonobo females overcome a phylogenetic legacy of male dominance.Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 157–179.Google Scholar
Parr, L. A., Matheson, M., Bernstein, I. S. & de Waal, F. B. M. (1997). Grooming down the hierarchy: allogrooming in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 54, 361–367.Google Scholar
Pereira, M. E. & Kappeler, P. M. (1997). Divergent systems of agonistic relationships in lemurs. Behaviour, 134, 225–274.Google Scholar
Pereira, M. E. & McGlynn, C. A. (1997). Special relationships instead of female dominance for redfronted lemurs, Eulemur fulvus rufus. American Journal of Primatology, 43, 239–258.Google Scholar
Pereira, M. E., Kaufmann, R., Kappeler, P. M. & Overdorff, D. J. (1990). Female dominance does not characterize all of the Lemuridae. Folia Primatologica, 55, 96–103.Google Scholar
Pinkus, S, Smith, J. N. M. & Jolly, A. (2006). Feeding competition between introduced Eulemur fulvus and native Lemur catta during the birth season at Berenty Reserve, Southern Madagascar. In: Jolly, A., Sussman, R. W., Koyama, N. and Rasamimanana, H. (eds), Ringtailed Lemur Biology. New York: Springer, pp. 119–140.
Pollock, J. I. (1979). Female dominance in Indri indri. Folia Primatologica, 31, 143–164.Google Scholar
Preuschoft, S. & van Schaik, C. P. (2000). Dominance and communication: conflict management in various social settings. In: Aureli, F and de Waal, F. B. M. (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 77–105.
Pusey, A., Williams, J. & Goodall, J. (1997). The influence of dominance rank on the reproductive success of female chimpanzees. Science, 277, 828–831.Google Scholar
Radespiel, U. & Zimmermann, E. (2001). Female dominance in captive mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus). American Journal of Primatology, 54, 181–192.Google Scholar
Ramanankirahina, R., Joly, M. & Zimmermann, E. (2011). Peaceful primates: affiliation, aggression, and the question of female dominance in a nocturnal pair-living lemur (Avahi occidentalis). American Journal of Primatology, 73(12), 1261–1268.Google Scholar
Raps, S. & White, F. J. (1995). Female social dominance in semi-free-ranging ruffed lemurs (Varecia variegata). Folia Primatologica, 65, 163–168.Google Scholar
Rasamimanana, H. R. & Rafidinarivo, E. (1993). Feeding behavior of Lemur catta females in relation to their physiological state. In: Kappeler, P. M. & Ganzhorn, J. U. (eds), Lemur Social Systems and their Ecological Basis. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 122–133.
Rathbun, G. B. (1979). The social structure and ecology of elephant-shrews. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20(Supplement), 1–76.Google Scholar
Rendall, D. (1993). Does female social precedence characterize captive aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis)? Folia Primatologica, 14, 125–130.Google Scholar
Richard, A. F. (1974). Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatologica, 22, 178–207.Google Scholar
Richard, A. F. & Nicoll, M. E. (1987). Female social dominance and basal metabolism in a malagasy primate, Propithecus verreauxi. American Journal of Primatology, 12, 309–314.Google Scholar
Roberts, G. (2005). Cooperation through interdependence. Animal Behaviour, 70, 901–908.Google Scholar
Rowell, T. E. (1967). A quantitative comparison of the behaviour of a wild and a caged baboon troop. Animal Behaviour, 15, 499–509.Google Scholar
Sakamaki, T., Behncke, I., Laporte, M.et al. (2015). Intergroup transfer of females and social relationships between immigrants and residents in bonobo (Pan paniscus) societies. Dispersing primate females. In: Furuichi, T., Yamagiwa, J. & Aureli, F. (eds) Dispersing Primate Females. pp. 127–164. Japan, Springer.
Sapolsky, R. M. (2005). The influence of social hierarchy on primate health. Science, 308, 648–652.Google Scholar
Sapolsky, R. M., Romero, L. M. & Munck, A. U. (2000). How do glucocorticoids influence stress responses? Integrating permissive, suppressive, stimulatory, and preparative actions. Endocrine Review, 21, 55–89.Google Scholar
Sauther, M. L. (1991). Reproductive behavior of free-ranging Lemur catta at Beza Mahafaly special reserve, Madagascar. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 84, 463–477.Google Scholar
Sauther, M. L. (1993). Resource competition in wild populations of ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta): Implications for female dominance. In: Kappeler, P. M. and Ganzhorn, J. (eds), Lemur Social Systems and Their Ecological Basis. New York: Plenum, pp. 135–152.
Savin-Williams, R. C. (1977). Dominance in a human adolescent group. Animal Behavior, 25, 400–406.Google Scholar
Savin-Williams, R. C. (1979). Dominance hierarchies in groups of early adolescents. Child Development, 50, 923–935.Google Scholar
Savin-Williams, R. C. (1980). Dominance hierarchies in groups of middle to late adolescent males. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 9, 75–85.Google Scholar
Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. (1922). Beiträge zur Sozialpsychologie des Haushuhns. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 88, 226–252.Google Scholar
Schryer, F. J. (2001). Multiple hierarchies and the duplex nature of groups. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 7, 705–721.Google Scholar
Sclafani, V., Norscia, I., Antonacci, D. & Palagi, E. (2012). Scratching around mating: factors affecting anxiety in wild Lemur catta. Primates, 53, 247–254.Google Scholar
Shizuka, D. & McDonald, D. B. (2012). A social network perspective on measurements of dominance hierarchies. Animal Behaviour, 83, 925–934.Google Scholar
Shizuka, D. & McDonald, D. B. (2014). Errata corrige to Shizuka, D., and McDonald, D. B. (Animal Behaviour, 83, 925–934). Animal Behaviour, 87, 243.Google Scholar
Simmen, B., Hladik, A. & Ramasiarisoa, P. (2003). Food intake and dietary overlap in native Lemur catta and Propithecus verreauxi and introduced Eulemur fulvus at Berenty, Southern Madagascar. International Journal of Primatology, 24, 949–968.Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, A., Marmot, M. G. & Adler, N. E. (2005). Does subjective social status predict health and change in health status better than objective status?Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 855–861.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. M. G., Vervaecke, H., de Vries, H. & van Elsacker, L. (2007). Sex differences in the steepness of dominance hierarchies in captive bonobo groups. International Journal of Primatology, 28, 1417–1430.Google Scholar
Strayer, F. F. & Strayer, J. (1976). An ethological analysis of social agonism and dominance relations among preschool children. Child Development, 47, 980–989.Google Scholar
Sueur, C., Petit, O., De Marco, A., et al. (2011). A comparative network analysis of social style in macaques. Animal Behaviour, 82, 845–852.Google Scholar
Surbeck, M., Mundry, R. & Hohmann, G. (2010). Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos (Pan paniscus). Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278, 590–598.Google Scholar
Tanaka, M. (2007). Habitat use and social structure of a brown lemur hybrid population in the Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. American Journal of Primatology, 69, 1189–1194.Google Scholar
Tecot, S. R. & Romine, N. K. (2012). Leading ladies: Leadership of group movements in a pair-living, co-dominant, monomorphic primate across reproductive stages and fruit availability seasons. American Journal of Primatology, 74, 591–601.Google Scholar
Thierry, B. (1986). A comparative study of aggression and response to aggression in three species of macaque. In: Else, J. G. & Lee, P. C. (eds), Primate Ontogeny, Cognition and Social BehaviourCambridge University Press, pp. 307–313.
Thierry, B. (2000). Covariation of conflict management patterns across macaque species. In: Aureli, F. & Waal, F. B. M. de (eds), Natural Conflict Resolution, Berkeley, CA: University of California Press, pp. 106–128.
Thierry, B. (2013). Identifying constraints in the evolution of primate societies. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368, 20120342.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. & Kappeler, P. M. (1996). The social systems of gregarious lemurs: lack of convergence with anthropoids due to evolutionary disequilibrium?Ethology, 102, 915–941.Google Scholar
Vehrencamp, S. L. (1983). A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies. Animal Behaviour, 31, 667–682.Google Scholar
von Holst, D., Hutzelmeyer, H., Kaetzke, P., et al. (2002). Social rank, fecundity and lifetime reproductive success in wild European rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 51, 245–254.Google Scholar
Waeber, P. O. & Hemelrijk, C. K. (2003). Female dominance and social structure in Alaotran gentle lemurs. Behaviour, 140, 1235–46.Google Scholar
Warkentin, M. J. (1968). Observations on the behavior and ecology of the nutria in Louisiana. Tulane Studies in Zoology and Botany, 15, 10–17.Google Scholar
Wasserman, S., Faust, K. & Iacobucci, D. (1994). Social Network Analysis: Methods and Applications (Structural Analysis in the Social Sciences). Cambridge University Press.
Webster, G. & Goodwin, B. C. (1982). The origin of species: a structuralist approach. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 5, 15–47.Google Scholar
White, F. J. (1988). Party composition and dynamics in Pan paniscus. International Journal of Primatology, 9, 179–193.Google Scholar
Wickings, E. J. & Dixson, A. F. (1992). Testicular function, secondary sexual development, and social status in male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx). Physiology and Behavior, 52, 909–916.Google Scholar
Wright, P. C. (1984). Biparental care in Aotus trivirgatus and Callicebus moloch. In: Small, M. (ed.), Female Primates: Studies by women primatologists. New York: Alan R. Liss, Inc., pp. 59–75.
Wright, P. C. (1993). Variations in male–female dominance and offspring care in non-human primates. In: Miller, B. D. (ed.), Sex and Gender Hierachies. Cambridge University Press, pp. 127–145.
Wright, P. C. (1999). Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 42, 31–72.Google Scholar
Wroblewski, E. E., Murray, M. C., Keele, F. B., et al. (2009). Male dominance rank and reproductive success in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii. Animal Behaviour, 77(4), 873–885.Google Scholar
Young, A. L., Richard, A. F. & Aiello, L. C. (1990). Female dominance and maternal investment in strepsirrhine primates. American Naturalist, 135, 473–488.Google Scholar
Zaramody, A., Fausser, J. L., Roos, C., et al. (2006). Molecular phylogeny and taxonomic revision of the eastern woolly lemurs (Avahi laniger). Primate Reports, 74, 9–22.Google Scholar
Zink, C. F., Tong, Y., Chen, Q., et al. (2008). Know your place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron, 58, 273–283.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×