Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:55:57.532Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - The neural correlates of reading disorder: functional magnetic resonance imaging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Juliana Paré-Blagoev
Affiliation:
Research Scientist The MIND Institute; Assistant Professor University of New Mexico Department of Psychology
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Jane Holmes Bernstein
Affiliation:
The Children's Hospital, Boston
Mary Helen Immordino-Yang
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Overview: Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) provides a powerful tool for analyzing brain correlates of reading problems, although conclusions must be qualified by methodological confounds that researchers need to examine. Theoretical analyses of diverse patterns of dyslexia suggest three different impairments in phonological, visual, or auditory skills. Recent research with fMRI indicates that distinct brain regions most likely show reduced activation for each of these problems. For children with phonological problems, reduction is in left temporo-parietal regions, which are involved in speech. For those with visual problems, reduction is in the magnocellular system, which is involved in vision. For those with temporal processing problems, reduction seems to include the frontal and left temporal areas.

The Editors

Ever since Dejerine's work implicated the left angular gyrus as playing a special role in impairments of reading and writing, neuroscientists have recognized the importance of understanding the neural underpinnings of dyslexia (Dejerine, 1891). The invention in the 1990s of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a non-invasive neuroimaging technique, allows unprecedented access to the human brain at work at spatial resolutions of millimeters and temporal resolutions of seconds. (Some analysis techniques push this resolution even lower.) Findings have mirrored differences found in morphological studies with prior techniques, and gone beyond them: on the order of a dozen different brain regions have been shown to have different blood flow patterns during reading-related tasks in dyslexics as compared to normal readers.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boder, E. (1973). Developmental dyslexia: A diagnostic approach based on three atypical reading-spelling patterns. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 15, 663–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read – a causal connection. Nature, 301, 419–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C., Frith, C. & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics – A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz?Brain, 122(10), 1901–917.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B., Davidson, M. & Rosen, B. (2002). Functional magnetic resonance imaging: Basic principles of and application to developmental science. Developmental Science, 5(3), 301–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castles, A. & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornelissen, P. L., Hansen, P. C., Hutton, J. L., Evangelinou, V. & Stein, J. (1998). Magnocellular visual function and children's single word reading. Vision Research, 38, 471–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornia, D., Richards, T., Serafini, S., Richards, A., Steury, K., Abbott, R., Echelard, D. R., Marvaliaa, K. R., & Berninger, V. W. (2001). fMRI auditory language differences between dyslexic and able reading children. Neuroreport, 12(6), 1195–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dejerine, J. (1891). Sur un cas de cécité verbale avec agraphie, suivi d'autopsie. Mémoires Société Biologique, 3, 197–201.Google Scholar
DeMartino, S., Espesser, R., Rey, V. & Habib, M. (2000). The ‘temporal processing deficit’ hypothesis of dyslexia: new experimental evidence. Brain and Cognition, 46(1–2), 104–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demb, J., Boynton, G. & Heeger, D. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of early visual pathways in dyslexia. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(17), 6939–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denenberg, V. (1999). A critique of Mody, Studdert-Kennedy and Brady's ‘Speech perception deficits in poor readers: auditory processing or phonological coding?Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 379–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature, 382, 66–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M. & Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). The visual deficit theory of developmental dyslexia. [Review]. Neuroimage, 4, S108–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eden, G. F. & Zeffiro, T. (1998). Neural systems affected in developmental dyslexia revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuron, 21, 279–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farmer, M. & Klein, R. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2(4), 460–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaillard, W., Hertz-Pannier, L., Mott, S., Barnett, A., LeBihan, D. & Theodore, W. (2000). Functional anatomy of cognitive development. fMRI of verbal fluency in children and adults. Neurology, 54(1), 180–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgiewa, P., Rzanny, R., Hopf, J., Knab, R., Glauche, V., Kaiser, W. & Blanz, B. (1999). fMRI during word processing in dyslexic and normal reading children. Neuroreport, 10, 3459–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goswami, U. (2002). Phonology, reading development and dyslexia: A cross-linguistic perspective. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: An update on genes, brain, and environments. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 91–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossglenn, K., Duara, R., Barker, W., Loewenstein, D., Chang, J., YoshiI, F., Apicella, A., Pascal, S., Boothe, T., Sevush, S., Jallad, B., Novoa, L. & Lubs, H. (1991). Positron emission tomographic studies during serial word-reading by normal and dyslexics adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13(4), 531–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habib, M. (2000). The neurological basis of developmental dyslexia: An overview and working hypothesis. Brain, 123, 2373–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habib, M., Espesser, R., Rey, V., Giraud, K., Bruas, P. & Gres, C. (1999). Training dyslexics with acoustically modified speech: Evidence of improved phonological performance. Brain and Cognition, 40(1), 143–6.Google Scholar
Hatcher, P., Hulme, C. & Ellis, A. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helenius, P., , A, T., Cornelissen, P. L., Hansen, P. C. & Salmelin, R. (1999). Dissociation of normal feature analysis and deficient processing of letter-strings in dyslexic adults. Cerebral Cortex, 9, 476–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hook, P., Macaruso, P. & Jones, S. (2001). Efficacy of Fast ForWord training on facilitation acquisition of reading skills by children with reading difficulties – A longitudinal study. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 75–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (2002). Neural plasticity: The effects of environment on the development of the cerebral cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Larsen, F., Hoien, T., Lundeberg, I. & Odegaard, H. (1990). MRI evaluation of the size and symmetry of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 289–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leonard, C. (1996). Structural variation in the developing and mature cerebral cortex: Noise or signal? In Thatcher, R., Lyon, G., Rumsey, J. & Krasnegor, N. (eds), Developmental Neuroimaging, 207–31. Orlando: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, I. (1973). Segmentation of the spoken word and reading acquisition. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 23, 65–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logothetis, N., Paulis, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T. & Oeltermann, A. (2001). Neurophysio logical investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, 412, 150–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovegrove, W., Bowling, A., Badcock, D. & Blackwood, M. (1980). Specific reading disability: differences in contrast sensitivity as a function of spatial frequency. Science, 210, 439–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovegrove, W., Heddle, M. & Slaghuis, W. (1980). Reading disability: spatial frequency specific deficits in visual information store. Neuropsychologia, 18, 111–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovegrove, W., Martin, F., Bowling, A., Blackwood, M., Badcock, D. & Paxton, S. (1982). Contrast sensitivity functions and specific reading disability. Neuro psychologia, 20, 309–15.Google ScholarPubMed
Lundberg, I., Frost, J. & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–12.Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., Olofsson, A. & Wall, S. (1980). Reading and spelling skills in the first school years predicted from phonemic awareness skills in kindergarten. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 21, 159–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manis, F., Custodio, R. & Szeszulski, P. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: A 2-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 64–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manis, F., Seidenberg, M., Doi, L., McBride-Chang, C. & Peterson, A. (1996). On the bases of two subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 58, 157–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson, J., Hazan, V. & Wijayatilake, L. (1995). Phonemic processing problems in developmental phonological dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 233–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAnally, D., Hansen, P., Cornelissen, P. & Stein, F. (1997). Effect of time and frequency manipulation on syllable perception in developmental dyslexics. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 912–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merzenich, M., Jenkins, W., Johnston, P., Schreiner, C., Miller, S. & Tallal, P. (1996). Temporal processing deficits of language-learning impaired children ameliorated by training. Science, 271, 77–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitterer, J. (1982). There are at least two kinds of poor readers: Whole word poor readers and recoding poor readers. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 36, 445–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mody, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Brady, S. (1997). Speech perception deficits in poor readers: Auditory processing or phonological coding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 199–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, L. & Pollatsek, A. (1994). Developmental dyslexia: Heterogeneity without discrete subgroups. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 120–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagarajan, S., Mahncke, H., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Roberts, T. & Merzenich, M. (1999). Cortical auditory signal processing in poor readers. Proceedings of National Academy of Science, USA, 96, 6483–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E., Frith, U., Snowling, M., Gallagher, A., Morton, J., Frackowiak, R. & Frith, C. (1996). Is developmental dyslexia a disconnection syndrome? Evidence from PET scanninng. Brain, 119, 143–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poldrack, R., Paré-Blagoev, E. & Grant, P. (2002). Pediatric fMRI: Progress and challenges. Topics in Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 13, 61–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poldrack, R., Temple, E., Protopapas, A., Nagarajan, S., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. & Gabrieli, J. (2001). Relations between the neural bases of dynamic auditory processing and phonological processing: Evidence from fMRI. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(5), 687–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Jenner, A., Katz, L., Frost, S., Lee, J., Shaywitz, S. & Shaywitz, B. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 479–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Fulbright, R., Constable, R., Skudlarski, P., Marchione, K., Jenner, A., Fletcher, J., Liberman, A., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Lacadie, C. & Gore, J. (2000). The angular gyrus in developmental dyslexia: Task-specific differences in functional connectivity within posterior cortex. Psychological Science, 11(1), 51–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S., Simmons, A., Andrew, C. & Bullmore, E. (2000). Functional frontalisation with age: Mapping neurodevelopmental trajectories with fMRI. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(1), 9–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruff, S., Cardebat, D., Marie, N. & Demonet, J. (2002). Enhanced response of the left frontal cortex to slowed down speech in dyslexia: an fMRI study. Neuroreport, 13(10), 1285–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumsey, J., Horwitz, B., Donohue, B., Nace, K., Maisog, J. & Andreason, P. (1999). A functional lesion in developmental dyslexia: Left angular gyral blood flow predicts severity. Brain and Language, 70, 187–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumsey, J. M., Nace, K., Donohue, B., Wise, D., Maisog, J. M. & Andreason, P. (1997). A positron emission tomographic study of impaired word recognition and phonological processing in dyslexic men. Archives of Neurology, 54(5), 562–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y. & Steinmets, H. (1995). In vivo evidence of structural brain asymmetry in musicians. Science, 267, 699–701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seymour, P. & MacGregor, C. (1984). Developmental dyslexia: A cogntivie experimental analysis of phonological, morphemic, and visual impairments. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 1, 43–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapleske, J., Rossell, S., Woodruss, P. & David, A. (1999). The planum temporale: a systematic, quantitative review of its structural, functional and clinical significance. Brain Research Reviews, 29, 26–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, B., Shaywitz, S., Pugh, K., Mencl, W., Fulbright, R., Skudlarski, P., Constable, R., Marchione, K., Fletcher, J., Lyon, G. & Gore, J. (2002). Disruption of post erior brain systems for reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Biological Psychiatry, 52(2), 101–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Fletcher, J. & Escobar, M. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls. Journal of American Medical Association, 264, 998–1002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Constable, R., Mencl, W., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J., Katz, L., Marchione, K., Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C. & Gore, J. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Proceedings of National Academy of Science, USA, 95, 2636–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simos, P., Breier, J., Fletcher, J., Foorman, B., Bergman, E., Fishbeck, K. & Papanicolaou, A. (2000). Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-work reading: a magnetic source imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 290, 61–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skottun, B. (2000). The magnocellular deficit theory of dyslexia: The evidence from contrast sensitivity. Vision Research, 40, 111–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, S., Flores, D. & Noll, D. (1998). Different neural circuits subserve reading before and after therapy for acquired dyslexia. Brain and Language, 62, 298–308.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Miller, S., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. & Merzenich, M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271(5), 81–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1973). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P. & Piercy, M. (1975). Developmental aphasia: The perception of brief vowels and extended stop consonants. Neuropsychologia, 13, 69–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tallal, P., Stark, R. & Mellits, E. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain and Language, 25, 314–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E. (2002). The developmental cognitive neuroscience approach to the study of developmental disorders. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(6), 771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Protopapas, A., Nagarajan, S., Salz, T., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2000). Disruption of the neural response to rapid acoustic stimuli in dyslexia: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97(25), 13907–912.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Temple, E., Poldrack, R. A., Salidis, J., Deutsch, G. K., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2001). Disrupted neural responses to phonological and orthographic processing in dyslexic children: An fMRI study. Neuroreport, 12(2), 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R., Torgesen, J. & Rashotte, C. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Miller, L. & Donnelly, K. (2000). Retrieval, automaticity vocabulary, elaboration-orthography (RAVE-O): A comprehensive, fluency-based reading intervention program. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×