Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T11:15:07.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - A brief history of time, phonology, and other explanations of developmental dyslexia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Maryanne Wolf
Affiliation:
Director of the Center for Reading and Language Research and Professor of Child Development Tufts University
Jane Ashby
Affiliation:
Researcher University of Massachusetts, Amherst
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Jane Holmes Bernstein
Affiliation:
The Children's Hospital, Boston
Mary Helen Immordino-Yang
Affiliation:
University of Southern California
Get access

Summary

Overview: Throughout the history of the study of reading disabilities, research has been plagued by the heterogeneity inherent in individuals with dyslexia and the apparent contradictions in findings that this variability produces. In this thematic review of historical and current approaches, the authors transform this view of variability to show how it is a key source of information about development of the normal reading process and sources of reading problems. The new emphasis on connecting mind, brain, and education requires relating neurological and behavioral aspects of dyslexia in its varied forms. In their work the authors focus especially on understanding how reading disability relates both to processes underlying the slow retrieval of names for common visual stimuli, such as letters, numbers, and colors, and also to phonological processes. They discuss the educational implications of their findings for the historical conflict between “whole language” and “phonological” approaches to reading instruction. Looking to the future, they suggest that only through considering the complexity of the reading process and the variability present in reading disability profiles can progress be made in unlocking the true nature of this disorder.

The Editors

The title of this paper, “A brief history of time, phonology, and other explanations of developmental dyslexia,” was meant not only to evoke the title of Stephen Hawking's book, A Brief History of Time (1988), but also to underscore the similarly Sisyphean nature of both endeavors.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. T., Dykman, R. A. & Gardner, M. Y. (1990). Counting rate, naming rate, phonological sensitivity and memory span: Major factors in dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 23, 325–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Badian, N. (1994). Do dyslexic and other poor readers differ in reading related cognitive skills?Reading and Writing, 6(1), 45–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badian, N. (1995). Predicting reading ability over the long-term: The changing roles of letter naming, phonological awareness and orthographic knowledge. Annals of Dyslexia: An Interdisciplinary Journal, XLV, 79–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Badian, N. (1996). Dyslexia: A validation of the concept at two age levels. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 29, 102–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakker, D. J. (1972). Temporal order in disturbed reading-development and neuropsychological aspects in normal and reading-retarded children. Rotterdam: Rotterdam University Press.Google Scholar
Bender, L. (1956). Problems in conceptualization and communication in children with developmental alexia. Proceedings of the American Psychobiological Association.Google ScholarPubMed
Benton, A. L. (1978). Some conclusions about dyslexia. In Benton, A. L. & Pearl, D. (eds), Dyslexia: An appraisal of current knowledge, 451–526. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berninger, V. & Richards, T. (2003). Brain literacy for educators. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Birch, H. G. (1962). Dyslexia and the maturation of visual function. In Money, J. (ed.), Reading disability: Progress and research needs in dyslexia, Ch. X. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.Google Scholar
Birch, H. G. & Belmont, L. (1964). Auditory-visual integration in normal and retarded readers. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 34, 852–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blachman, B. A. (1994). What we have learned from longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading, and some unanswered questions: A response to Torgesen, Wagner, and Rashotte. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 287–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blank, M., Berenzweig, S. S. & Bridger, W. H. (1975). The effect of stimulus complexity and sensory modality on reaction time in normal and retarded readers. Child Development, 46, 133–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, P. G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Predictors of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowers, P. G., Golden, J., Kennedy, A. & Young, A. (1994). The varieties of orthographic knowledge. Vol. I: Theoretical and developmental issues. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R. & Tate, E. (1988). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 304–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read-a casual connection. Nature, 301, 419–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brady, S. & Shankweiler, D. (1991). Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Brady, S., Shankweiler, D. & Mann, V. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 345–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breznitz, Z. & Berman, L. (2003). The underlying factors of word reading rate. Educational Psychology Review, 15(3), 247–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, B. (1998). The foundation of literacy: The child's acquisition of the alphabetic principle. East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cattell, M. (1886). The time it takes to see and name objects. Mind, 2, 63–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catts, H. W. (1996). Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder: An expanded view. Topics in Language Disorders, 16(2), 14–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Chase, C. (1996). Developmental dyslexia: Neural, cognitive, and genetic mechanisms. Baltimore, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Chase, C. & Jenner, A. R. (1993). Magnocellular visual deficits affect temporal processing of dyslexics. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 326–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clements, S. D. & Peters, J. E. (1962). Minimal brain dysfunction in the school-aged child: Diagnosis and treatment. Archives of General Psychiatry, 6(3), 185–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Critchley, M. (1964). Developmental dyslexia. London: Heineman Medical Books Limited.Google Scholar
Dejerine, J. (1892). Contribution à l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différents variétés de cécité verbale. Paper presented at the Comptes Rendu des Séances. Société de Biologie et de Ses Filiales et Associées, Paris.Google Scholar
Demb, J., Poldrack, R. & Gabrieli, J. (1999). Functional neuroimaging of word processing in normal and dyslexic readers. In Klein, R. & MacMullen, P. A. (eds), Converging methods for understanding reading and dsylexia, 245–304. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1974). “Rapid automatized naming” of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1976a). Naming of objects by dyslexic and other learning-disabled children. Brain and Language, 3, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denckla, M. B. & Rudel, R. G. (1976b). Rapid automatized naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eden, G. F. & Zeffiro, T. A. (1998). Neural systems affected in developmental dyslexia revealed by functional neuroimaging. Neuron, 21, 279–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felton, R. H. (1993). Effects of instruction on the decoding skills of children with phonological-processing problems. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 583–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felton, R. H. & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing, 2, 39–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fildes, L. G. (1921). A psychological inquiry into the nature of the condition known as congenital word-blindness. Brain, 44, 286–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foorman, B., Francis, D., Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B. & Fletcher, J. (1998). The case for early reading intervention. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Foorman, B., Francis, D., Winikates, D., Mehta, P., Schatschneider, C. & Fletcher, J. (1997). Early intervention for children with reading disabilities. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1(3), 255–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T. & Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91, 8010–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geschwind, N. (1965). Disconnection syndrome in animals and man (Parts I, II). Brain, 88, 237–94, 585–644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1974). Selected papers on language and the brain. Dordrecht, Holland: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geschwind, N. (1982). Why Orton was right. Annals of Dyslexia, 32, 13–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Hanson, V. L., Liberman, I. Y. & Shankweiler, D. P. (1984). Linguistic coding by deaf children in relation to beginning reading success. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37(2), 378–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawking, S. (1988). A brief history of time. Toronto: Bantam.Google Scholar
Heim, S. & Keil, A. (2004). Large-scale neural correlates of developmental European dyslexia. Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 13, 125–40.Google Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1895). Word-blindness and visual memory. Lancet, 2, 1564–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1900). Congenital word-blindness. Lancet, 1, 1506–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinshelwood, J. (1917). Congenital word-blindness. London: Lewis.Google Scholar
Ho, C., Tsang, S.-M. & Lee., S. H. (2002). The cognitive profile of multiple deficit hypothesis in Chinese. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huey, E. B. (1908). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Johnson, D. J. & Myklebust, H. R. (1964). Learning Disabilities. New York: Grune and Stratton.Google Scholar
Kame'enui, E., Simmons, D., Good III, R. & Harn, B. (2001). The use of fluency-based measures in early identification and evaluation of intervention efficacy in schools. In Wolf, M. (ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain, 307–31. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Kamhi, A. & Catts, H. (1989). Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Austin, TX: Pro-ed.Google Scholar
Kirby, J., Parilla, R. & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 453–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R. & McMullen, P. A. (eds) (1999). Converging methods for understanding reading and dyslexia. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Korhonen, T. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: A nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28, 232–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kussmaul, A. (1877). Disturbance of speech. In Ziemssen, H. (ed.), Encyclopedia of the practice of medicine, 14, translated by J. A. McCreery. New York: William Wood.
Liberman, I. Y. (1971). Basic research in speech and lateralization of language: Some implications for reading disability. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 21, 71–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D. P., Fischer, F. W. & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18(2), 201–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W. & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Neurobiology, 88, 7943–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Llinas, R. (1996). Presentation to Mind, Brain, and Behavior Subgroup on Development, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, April, 2006.
Lovegrove, W. J. & Williams, M. C. (1993). Visual processes in reading and reading disabilities. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia: An interdisciplinary journal, XⅬV, 3–27.Google Scholar
McBride-Chang, C. & Manis, F. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing, 8, 323–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, M. S. & Felton, R. H. (1998). Evolution of fluency training: Old approaches and new directions. Paper presented at the meeting of International Dyslexia Association (to appear in Annals of Dyslexia).
Misra, M., Katzir, T., Wolf, M. & Poldrack, R. (2004). Neural systems for RAN in skilled readers: Unraveling the RAN-Reading relationship. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 241–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, W. P. (1896). A case of congenital word-blindness. British Medical Journal, 2, 1378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, R., Lovett, M. & Wolf, M. (1996). Treatment of developmental reading disabilities. NICHD Grant Proposal.
Morris, R., Stuebing, K., Fletcher, J., Shaywitz, S., Lyon, R., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Francis, D. & Shaywitz, B. (1998). Subtypes of reading disability: A phonological core with cognitive variability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 1–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myklebust, H. R., Boshes, B., Olson, D. A. & Cole, C. H. (1969). Final Report: Minimal brain damage in children. Washington, DC: US Department of Health, Education, and Welfare.Google Scholar
National Reading Panel (2000). Summary report. Bethesda, MD: NICHD.
Novoa, L. (1988). Word-retrieval processes and reading acquisition and development in bilingual and monolingual children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University.
Ojemann, G. A. (1983). Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping. Behavioral Brain Science, 6, 189–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Connors, F., Rack, J. P. & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orton, S. T. (1928). Specific reading disability – Strephosymbolia. Journal of the American Medical Association, 90, 1095–1099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Parilla, R., Kirby, J. R. & McQuarrie, L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development?Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 3–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R. A. (2002). Neural systems for perceptual skill learning. Behavioral & Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1(1), 76–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I. & Raichle, M. E. (1995). Images of mind. New York, NY: W.H. Freeman & Company.Google Scholar
Rabinovitch, R. D. (1959). Reading and learning disabilities. In S. Arieti, (ed.), American handbook of psychiatry, 1, 857–69. New York: Basic Books, Inc.Google Scholar
Robinson, H. M. (1946). Why pupils fail in reading. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rosen, G., Fitch, R. H., Clark, M., Lo Turco, J. J., Sherman, G. & Galaburda, A. (2001). Animal models of developmental dyslexia. In Wolf, M. (ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain, 129–57. Timonium, MD: York Press.
Schilder, P. (1944). Congenital alexia and its relation to optic perception. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 65, 67–88.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D. & Liberman, I. Y. (1972). Language by ear and by eye. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shankweiler, D. P. & Liberman, I. Y. (eds). (1989). Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M. & Shaywitz, B. A. (1996). A conceptual model and definition of dyslexia: Findings emerging from the Connecticut Longitudinal Study. In Beitchman, J. E., Cohen, N. J., Konstantaseas, M. M. & Tannock, R. (eds), Language, learning, and behavior disorders: Developmental, biological and clinical perspectives, 199–223. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S., Shaywitz, B., Pugh, K., Fulbright, R., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A., Skudlarski, P., Fletcher, J., Katz, L., Marchione, K., Lacadie, C., Gatenby, C. & Gore, J. (1998). Functional disruption in the organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Neurobiology, 95, 2636–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Siegel, L. S. & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical sensitivity, phonological and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24(1), 28–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spring, C. & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanovich, K. E. (1986). “Matthew effects” in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 4, 360–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Concepts in developmental theories of reading skill: Cognitive resources, automaticity and modularity. Developmental Review, 10, 72–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (1994). Are discrepancy-based definitions of dyslexia empirically defensible? In Bos, K. P. (ed.), Current Directions in Dyslexia Research, 15–30. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(10), 276–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C. & Alexander, A. (2001). Principles of fluency instruction in reading. In Wolf, M. (ed.), Dyslexia, fluency and the brain, 333–55. Timonium, MD: York Press.
Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C. & Wagner, R. (1998). Research on instructional interventions for children with reading disabilities. Paper presented at the Society for Scientific Study of Reading, San Diego, CA.
Tunmer, W. (1995). Intervention strategies for developing onset-rime sensitivity and analogical transfer in reading disabled children. Paper presented at the Extraordinary Brain III Conference, Kauai, Hawaii.
Tunmer, W. E. & Nesdale, A. R. (1985). Phonemic segmentation skill and beginning reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 417–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turkeltaub, P. E., Gareau, L., Flowers, D. L., Zeffiro, T. A. & Eden, G. F. (2003). Development of neural mechanisms for reading. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 767–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bos, K. (1998). IQ, phonological sensitivity, and continuous-naming speed related to Dutch children's performance on two word identification tests. Dyslexia, 4, 73–89.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R. (1979). Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vellutino, F. R. & Scanlon, D. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321–63.Google Scholar
Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K. & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional casuality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M. (1979). The relationship of disorders of word-finding and reading in children and aphasics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.
Wolf, M. (ed.) (2001). Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Wolf, M. & Bowers, P. (1999). The “Double-Deficit Hypothesis” for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Bowers, P. & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolf, M. & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its interventions. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 211–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Pfeil, C., Lotz, R. & Biddle, K. (1994). Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors. In Berninget, V. E. (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, 1: Theoretical and developmental issues. Neuropsychology and Cognition, vol. 8.New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolf, M., Miller, L. & Donnelly, K. (2000). Retrieval, automaticity, vocabulary, elaboration-orthography (RAVE-O): A comprehensive, fluency-based reading intervention program. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 322–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P., Michel, G. & Ovrut, M. (1990). Rate variables and automatized naming in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 556–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wood, F. (1995). Naming speed deficits in dyslexia. Symposium paper presented at the Society for Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.
Young, A. & Bowers, P. G. (1995). Individual differences and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 428–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zurif, E. B. & Carson, G. (1970). Dyslexia in relation to cerebral dominance and temporal analysis. Neuropsychologia, 8, 351–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×