Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-mlc7c Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T21:27:22.775Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Observing the dynamics of construction: Children building bridges and new ideas

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
Part-Time Professor Graduate School of Education, University of Massachusetts, Boston; Classroom Teacher and Science Resource Teacher for Brookline Public School Brookline, Massachusetts
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

Perhaps the most useful element of the microdevelopmental approach is its demand for observations that are as direct and as continuous as possible. This use of evidence is in keeping with the successful practices of the natural sciences. Direct and continuous observations have provided the basis for most of the useful ideas of these sciences. What sort of notion would we have about cells if their structures and functions were not closely observed? Weren't centuries of careful observation required before the basic form and dynamics of the solar system were understood? For explanations to be valid in science they must be logical and arise from observations that are as close to the phenomena as possible. It is this emphasis on close observation that gives the microdevelopment approach its great explanatory potential.

Surprisingly, developmental psychology has made only limited use of this power of observation that the natural sciences depend on for so much. The microdevelopmental study described here attempted to reconnect developmental psychology to this potential by directly and continuously observing changes in fifth- and seventh-graders' conversations and actions as they worked in their everyday classroom. (The fifth-graders ranged from 10.8 to 12.1 years of age; the seventh-graders were between 12.7 and 13.7 years old.) During the task pairs of students built marshmallow-and-toothpick bridges across an eleven-inch (28 cm) gap between tables. Two lines of evidence, student conversations and actions, were then used to infer the children's lesson-related ideas and the mechanisms that built these ideas.

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 157 - 180
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Fischer, K. W. (1980). A theory of cognitive development: The control and construction of hierarchies of skills. Psychological Review, 87, 477–531CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Granott, N. (1995). Beyond one-dimensional change: Parallel concurrent distributed processes in learning and development. Human Development, 38, 302–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1993). Microdevelopment of co-construction of knowledge during problem-solving: Puzzled minds, weird creatures, and wuggles. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA [on line]. Available: http://theses.mit.edu:80/Dienst/UI/2.0/Composite/0018.mit.theses/1993-170/I?nsections=19
Granott, N., Fischer, K. W., & Parziale, J. (this volume). Bridging to the unknown: A transition mechanism in learning and development
James, W. (1907). Pragmatism and four essays from the meaning of truth. New York: World Publishing
James, W. (1958). Talks to teachers. New York: Norman
Parziale, J. (1995). Microdevelopment during an activity based science lesson: A pilot study. Unpublished qualifying paper. Harvard University, Cambridge, MA
Parziale, J., & Fischer, K. W. (1998). The practical use of skill theory in the classroom. In R. J. Sternberg & W. M. Williams (Eds.), Intelligence, instruction and assessment (pp. 95–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Perkins, D. N. (1991). Schools of thought: The necessary shape of education. Unpublished manuscript, Project Zero, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA
Piaget, J. (1952/1974). The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, trans.). Madison, CT: International Universities Press
Teasley, S. D., & Roschelle, J. (1993). Constructing a joint problem space: The computer as a tool for sharing knowledge. In S. P. Lajoie & S. J. Derry (Eds.), Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 229–257). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental structures. New York: International Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×