Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T01:44:49.713Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Microdevelopment and dynamic systems: Applications to infant motor development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Esther Thelen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Indiana University
Daniela Corbetta
Affiliation:
Department of HKLS & Department of Psychological Sciences Purdue University
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

For many years, the primary occupation of developmental psychologists has been to document the ages at which infants and children reached various cognitive, social, and perceptual-motor milestones. One legacy of Gesell and of Piaget (although not necessarily endorsed by these pioneers) has been elaborate catalogues of “ages and stages” by which to gauge developmental progress and to compare children from different cultures, backgrounds, and family or medical circumstances. A more recent corollary of this tradition is the concern with documenting the earliest evidence of a particular competence. Do three-month-old infants have the object concept? When can babies count?

While we can hardly imagine a developmental psychology not anchored by “age” on the x-axis, a different approach has surfaced in the last decade or so, and is now the topic of this volume. The focus has shifted from “when” to “how.” Microdevelopment is the study of the processes of change, not only the endpoints. It represents a shift in thinking, but also in methods. And it requires new theory, or at least new looks at old theory.

We contend in this chapter that the study of motor development in infancy has been a particularly successful example of microdevelopmental thinking and methods. There are two reasons why this is so. First, the particular theoretical stance that has gained wide acceptance in this domain – dynamic systems – provides both rationale and means for such a genuinely process approach.

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 59 - 79
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1996). The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 502–522Google ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, D., & Thelen, E. (1999). Lateral biases and fluctuations in infants’ spontaneous arm movements and reaching. Developmental Psychobiology, 34, 237–2553.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagard, J., & Pézé, A. (1997). Age changes in interlimb coupling and the development of bimanual coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29, 199–208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flament, F. (1975). Coordination et prévalence manuelle chez le nourrisson. Paris: Editions du C.N.R.S.
Gesell, A. (1939). Reciprocal interweaving in neuromotor development. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 70, 161–180CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gesell, A. (1956). The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In L. Carmichael (Ed.), Manual of child psychology (pp. 295–331). New York: John Wiley
Gesell, A., & Ames, L. B. (1947). The development of handedness. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 70, 155–175Google ScholarPubMed
Goldfield, E. (1993). Dynamic systems in development: Action systems. In L. B. Smith & E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 51–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Goldfield, E. C., & Michel, G. F. (1986a). Spatiotemporal linkage in infant interlimb coordination. Developmental Psychology, 19, 259–264CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldfield, E. C., & Michel, G. F. (1986b). The ontogeny of infant bimanual reaching during the first year. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 81–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1998). Unit of analysis in transit: From the individual's knowledge to the ensemble process. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5, 42–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofsten, C. (1991). Structuring of early reaching movements: A longitudinal study. Journal of Motor Behavior, 23, 280–292CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofsten, C., & Fazel-Zandy, S. (1984). Development of visually guided hand orientation in reaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 208–219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockman, J. J., Ashmead, D. H., & Bushnell, E. W. (1984). The development of anticipatory hand orientation during infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 37, 176–186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGraw, M. (1935). Growth: A study of Johnny and Jimmy. New York: Appleton-Century Croft
McGraw, M. (1945). The neuromuscular maturation of the human infant. New York: Columbia University Press
Newell, K. M., Scully, D. M., McDonald, P. V., & Baillargeon, R. (1989). Task constraints and infant grip configurations. Developmental Psychobiology, 22, 817–832CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prechtl, H. F. R. (1977). The neurological examination of the full-term newborn infant. London: S.I.M.P and Heinemann
Rochat, P. (1992). Self-sitting and reaching in 5- to 8-month-old infants: The impact of posture and its development on early eye-hand coordination. Journal of Motor Behavior, 24, 210–220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochat, P., & Bullinger, A. (1994). Posture and functional action in infancy. In A. Vyt, H. Bloch, & M. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Early child development in the French tradition (pp. 15–34). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1991). The “memory system” of prelinguistic infants. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive functions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 608, 517–536CrossRef
Rovee-Collier, C. K., Morongiello, B. A., Aron, M., & Kupersmidt, J. (1978). Topographic response differentiation and reversal in 3-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 323–333CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children's thinking. New York: Oxford University Press
Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Spencer, J. P., & Thelen, E. (2000). Spatially specific changes in infants’ muscle co-activity as they learn to reach. Infancy, 1, 275–302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, J. P., Vereijken, B., Diedrich, F. J., & Thelen, E. (2000). Posture and the emergence of manual skills. Developmental Science, 3, 216–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. (1986). Treadmill-elicited stepping in seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 57, 1498–1506CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. (1994). Three-month-old infants can learn task-specific patterns of interlimb coordination. Psychological Science, 5, 280–285CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., Bradshaw, G., & Ward, J. A. (1981). Spontaneous kicking in month-old infants: Manifestation of a human central locomotor program. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 32, 45–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., Kamm, K., Spencer, J. P., Schneider, K., & Zernicke, R. F. (1993). The transition to reaching: Mapping intention and intrinsic dynamics. Child Development, 64, 1058–1098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Corbetta, D., & Spencer, J. (1996). Development of reaching during the first year: Role of movement speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1059–1076Google ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1982). Newborn stepping: An explanation for a “disappearing reflex.”Developmental Psychology, 18, 760–775CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1983a). From spontaneous to instrumental behavior: Kinematic analysis of movement changes during very early learning. Child Development, 54, 129–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Fisher, D. M. (1983b). The organization of spontaneous leg movements in newborn infants. Journal of Motor Behavior, 15, 353–377CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., Ridley-Johnson, R., & Fisher, D. M. (1983). Shifting patterns of bilateral coordination and lateral dominance in the leg movements of young infants. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 29–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1997). Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & R. M. Lerner (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development (5th edn., pp. 563–634). New York: Wiley
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56 (Serial No. 223)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., Ulrich, B. D., & Niles, D. (1987). Bilateral coordination in human infants: Stepping on a split-belt treadmill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 405–410Google ScholarPubMed
Touwen, B. (1976). Neurological development in infancy. London: S.I.M.P. and Heinemann
Van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems and development. Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Harvester Wheatsheaf
Vereijken, B., & Thelen, E. (1997). Training infant treadmill stepping: The role of individual pattern stability. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 89–1023.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×