Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T20:40:37.161Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Macro- and microdevelopmental research: Assumptions, research strategies, constraints, and utilities

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Kang Lee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Queen's University, Kingston
Annette Karmiloff-Smith
Affiliation:
Neurocognitive Development Unit, University College London
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Over the last half century, developmental research has undergone tremendous change. Our knowledge of how children develop in the physical, neurological, cognitive, and social domains has advanced greatly. In some areas, progress has been so extensive that old theories have been abandoned and textbooks completely rewritten (e.g., infant perceptual and cognitive development). Other advances are evident in the emergence of entire fields that did not even exist two or three decades ago (e.g., developmental neuroscience: Johnson, 1999; developmental connectionism: Elman, Bates, Johnson, Karmiloff-Smith, Parisi, & Plunkett, 1996). Still other, relatively “old” areas have moved in novel directions that have revealed new facts about how children develop (e.g., motor skills: Thelen, 1995; language: Markman, 1991; memory: Bruck & Ceci, 1999).

Theoretical approaches to developmental research have also changed dramatically in the past fifty years. For instance, in cognitive development research, radical behaviorism was replaced by Piagetian constructivism in the 1950s and 1960s, which itself was challenged by symbolic information processing theorists in the 1970s. The assumptions of symbolic information processing theories were later contested by connectionist theorists and domain-specificity theorists in the 1980s and 1990s. Nativist theories are also again in vogue after being marginalized for nearly a century. In addition, dynamic systems approaches derived from chaos theory in mathematics and thermodynamic research have been introduced to account for certain patterns of child development (Thelen & Smith, 1994; van Geert, 1991).

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 243 - 266
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bruck, M., & Ceci, S. J. (1999). The suggestibility of children's memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 419–439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catan, L. (1986). The dynamic display of process: Historical development and contemporary uses of the microgenetic method. Human Development, 29, 252–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooney, J. B., & Troyer, R. (1994). A dynamic model of reaction time in a short-term memory task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 200–226CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fischer, K. W., & Granott, N., (1995). Beyond one-dimensional change: Parallel, concurrent, socially distributed processes in learning and development. Human Development, 38, 302–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogel, A., & Thelen, E. (1987). Developmental early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1998a). A paradigm shift in the study of development. Human Development, 41, 360–365CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1998b). Unit of analysis in transit: From the individual's knowledge to the ensemble process. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 5, 42–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. H. (1999). Developmental neuroscience. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (4th edn.) (pp. 199–230). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). Micro- and macrodevelopmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems. Cognitive Science, 3, 91–117CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1981). Getting developmental differences or studying child development?Cognition, 10, 151–158CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1985). From metaprocesses to conscious access: Evidence from children's metalinguistic and repair data. Cognition, 23, 95–147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1993a). Self-organization and cognitive change. In M. H. Johnson (Ed.), Brain development and cognition: A reader (pp. 532–618). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1993b). Beyond Piaget's “epistemic subject”: Inhelder's microgenetic study of the “psychological subject.”Archives de Psychologie, 61, 247–252Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Inhelder, B. (1974). If you want to get ahead, get a theory. Cognition, 3, 195–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D. (1995). Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us?Psychological Science, 6, 133–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Schauble, L., & Garcia-Mila, M. (1992). Cross-domain development of scientific reasoning. Cognition and Instruction, 9, 285–327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38, 71–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (this volume). Interacting time scales in personality (and cognitive) development: Intentions, emotions, and emergent forms
Markman, E. M. (1991). The whole-object, taxonomic, and mutual exclusivity assumptions as initial constraints on word meanings. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Byrnes (Eds.), Perspectives on language and thought: Interrelations in development (pp. 72–106). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Rabinowitz, F. M., Grant, M. J., Howe, M. L., & Walsh, C. (1994). Reasoning in middle childhood: A dynamic model of performance on transitivity tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 252–288CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S. (1995). Children's thinking: How does change occur? In F. E. Weinert & W. Schneider (Eds.), Memory performance and competencies: Issues in growth and development (pp. 405–430). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Siegler, R. S. (1997). Concepts and methods for studying cognitive change. In E. Amsel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Change and development: Issues of theory, method, and application. The Jean Piaget symposium series (pp. 77–97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (1998). Developmental differences in rule learning: A microgenetic analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 273–310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S., & Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: A direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46, 606–620CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (Eds.) (1993). A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development: A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 79–95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (this volume). Microdevelopment and dynamic systems: Applications to infant motor development
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56 (Serial No. 223), 1–35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Geert, P. (this volume). Developmental dynamics, intentional action, and fuzzy sets.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×