Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T01:38:03.116Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Developmental dynamics, intentional action, and fuzzy sets

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Paul van Geert
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Groningen
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

One of the most striking findings of microdevelopmental studies is the existence of a considerable degree of variability in a person's performance levels (Crowley, Shrager, & Siegler, 1997; Fletcher, Huffman, Bray, & Grupe, 1998; Kuhn, Garcia-Mila, Zohar, & Andersen, 1995; Kuhn, 1995; Nakatani, 1995; Siegler, 1995). In addition, variability itself seems to vary between persons and conditions too (Alibali, 1999; Goldin-Meadow, Alibali, & Church, 1993). Still another finding is that individuals differ among one another in the way they reach a developmental outcome or in the way they show the psychological property under scrutiny (for instance, the use of a particular cognitive strategy) (Siegler, 1994, 1995, 1997; Siegler & Shipley, 1995; Siegler & Chen, 1998; Lautrey, 1993; Lautrey & Caroff, 1996).

The standard view on variability – which I shall confine to fluctuations in a person's level across time – is that it is more closely related to the notions of error and deception than to the idea of truth. The psychologist is like Narcissus, who looks at his face mirrored by the water of a pond that is touched by a gale of wind. The wrinkles he sees are those of the water, not of his face. If we study a property such as a person's level of skill, knowledge, intelligence – all alleged internal properties – we look at that property through some medium, for instance a test, an observation of spontaneous behavior, and so forth.

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 319 - 343
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alibali, M. W. (1999). How children change their minds: Strategy change can be gradual or abrupt. Developmental Psychology, 35, 127–145CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altrock, C. von (1995). Fuzzy logic and neurofuzzy applications explained. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall
Castro, J. L. (1995). Fuzzy logic controllers are universal approximators. IEEETransactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 25, 629–635CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crowley, K., Shrager, J., & Siegler, R. S. (1997). Strategy discovery as a competitive negotiation between metacognitive and associative mechanisms. Developmental Review, 17, 462–489CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weerth, C., & Geert, P. (1998). Emotional instability as an indicator of strictly timed infantile developmental transitions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 15–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weerth, C., Geert, P., & Hoijtink, H. (1999). Intraindividual variability in infant behavior. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1102–1112CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, J. L. (1990). Representation and structure in connectionist models. In G. T. M. Altmann (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 345–382). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Fletcher, K. L., Huffman, L. F., Bray, N. W., & Grupe, L. A. (1998). The use of the microgenetic method with children with disabilities: Discovering competence. Early Education and Development, 9, 357–373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M. W., & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100, 279–297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greeno, J. G. (1997). Theories and practices of thinking and learning to think. American Journal of Education, 106, 85–126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greeno, J. G. (1998). The situativity of knowing, learning, and research. American Psychologist, 53, 5–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosenfeld, B., Maas, H. L. J., & Boom, D. C. (1997a). Indicators of discontinuous change in the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 367–395CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosenfeld, B., Maas, H. L. J., & Boom, D. C. (1997b). Detecting bimodality in the analogical reasoning performance of elementary schoolchildren. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 529–547CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosko, B. (1993). Fuzzy thinking: the new science of fuzzy logic. New York: Hyperion
Kosko, B. (1997). Fuzzy engineering. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall
Kuhn, D. (1995). Microgenetic study of change: What has it told us?Psychological Science, 6, 133–139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, D., Garcia-Mila, M., Zohar, A., & Andersen, C. (1995). Strategies of knowledge acquisition. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60 (Serial No. 245)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lautrey, J. (1993). Structure and variability: A plea for a pluralistic approach to cognitive development. In R. Case & W. Edelstein (Eds.), The new structuralism in cognitive development: Theory and research on individual pathways (pp. 101–114). Basel: Karger
Lautrey, J., & Caroff, X. (1996). Variability and cognitive development. Polish Quarterly of Developmental Psychology, 2, 71–89Google Scholar
Nakatani, K. (1995). Microgenesis of the length perception of paired lines. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 58, 75–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nguyen, H. T., & Walker, E. A. (1997). A first course in fuzzy logic. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press
Ross, T. J. (2000). Fuzzy logic with engineering applications. New York: McGraw-Hill
Ruhland, R., & Geert, P. (1998). Jumping into syntax: Transitions in the development of closed class words. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 65–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savelsbergh, G., Maas, H. van der, & Geert, P. L. C. van (1999). Non-linear developmental processes. New York: Elsevier
Siegler, R. S. (1994). Cognitive variability: A key to understanding cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 1–5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1995). How does change occur: A microgenetic study of number conservation. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 225–273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S. (1997). Beyond competence – toward development. Cognitive Development, 12, 323–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (1998). Developmental differences in rule learning: A microgenetic analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 36, 273–310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Siegler, R. S., & Shipley, C. (1995). Variation, selection, and cognitive change. In T. J. Simon & G. S. Halford (Eds.), Developing cognitive competence: New approaches to process modeling (pp. 31–76). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
van der Maas, H. L. (1993). Catastrophe analysis of stagewise cognitive development. Amsterdam: Faculty of Psychology, University of Amsterdam (academic dissertation)
Maas, H. L. J., & Hopkins, B. (1998). Developmental transitions: So what's new?British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maas, H. L., & Molenaar, P. C. (1992). Stagewise cognitive development: An application of catastrophe theory. Psychological Review, 99, 395–417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Geert, P. (1997). Variability and fluctuation: A dynamic view. In E. Amsel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Change and development: Issues of theory, method, and application (pp. 193–212). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
van Geert, P. (2000). Time, change and intentional action in a dynamic framework: On the nature of psychological causes and measurement. Unpublished manuscript, University of Groningen
van Geert, P. L. C., Savelsbergh, G., & van der Maas, H. (1999). Transitions and non-linear dynamics in developmental psychology. In G. Savelsbergh, H. van der Maas, & P. L. C. van Geert (Eds.), Non-linear developmental processes (pp. 11–20). New York: Elsevier

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×