Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-8bhkd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T16:10:32.930Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2014

Helmut Schnelle
Affiliation:
Ruhr-Universität, Bochum, Germany
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Language in the Brain , pp. 213 - 218
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbib, M.A. (2002). Grounding the mirror system hypothesis for the evolution of the language ready brain. In Cangelosi, A., and Parisi, D., editors. Simulating the Evolution of Language. Springer, Berlin.Google Scholar
Arbib, M.A. (2003). Language evolution: the mirror system hypothesis. In Arbib, M.A., editor. Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Network, 2nd edition. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Arbib, M.A., Erdi, P., and Szentagothai, J. (1998). Neural Organization: Structure, Function, and Dynamics. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual Thinking. University of California Press, Berkeley CA.Google Scholar
Arnheim, R. (1971). Entropy and Art. University of California Press, Berkeley: CA.Google Scholar
Baars, B.J. (2007a). A framework. In Baars, B.J., and Gage, N.M., editors. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness – Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. Academic Press, London, pp. 31–58.Google Scholar
Baars, B.J. (2007b). Language. In Baars, B.J., and Gage, N.M., editors. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness – Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. Academic Press, London, pp. 317–341.Google Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Y. (1950). On syntactical categories. Journal of Symbolic Logic 15:1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Hillel, Y. (1954). Logical syntax and semantics. Language 30:230–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D. (2002). Fractionating the central executive. In Stuss, D.T., and Knight, R.T., editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, Oxford/ New York, pp. 246–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben Shalom, D., and Poeppel, D. (2008). Functional anatomic models of language: assembling the pieces. Neuroscientist 14:119–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S-J., and Frith, U. (2005). The Learning Brain. Blackwell, Malden MA.Google ScholarPubMed
Brauer, J., Neumann, J., and Friederici, A. (2008). Temporal dynamics of perisylvian activation during language processing in children and adults. Neuroimage 41:1484–1492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s Talk – Learning to Use Language. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Cannon, W.B. (1939). The Wisdom of the Body. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
Caplan, D. (1999). Language and communication. In Zigmond, M.J., Bloom, F.E., Landis, S.C., Roberts, J.L., and Squire, L.R., editors. Fundamental Neuroscience. Academic Press, San Diego, pp. 1499–1502.Google Scholar
Carnap, R. (1937). The Logical Syntax of Language. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
Carnap, R. (2003). The Logical Structure of the World. Open Court, Chicago, Ill. (1928 German ed.).Google Scholar
Chafe, W.L. (1973). Language and memory. Language 49:261–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W.L. (1974). Language and consciousness. Language 50:111–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changeux, J.P., and Danchin, A. (1976). Selective stabilisation of the developing synapses as a mechanism for the specification of neural networks. Nature 264:705–712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. Mouton, The Hague.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1963a). Introduction to formal analysis of natural languages. In Luce, R.D., Bush, R.R., and Galanter, E., editors. Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. John Wiley, New York, pp. 269–321.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1963b). Formal properties of grammars. In Luce, R.D., Bush, R.R., and Galanter, E., editors. Handbook of Mathematical Psychology. John Wiley, New York, pp. 326–418.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (2000). Linguistics and brain science. In Marantz, A., Mijashita, Y., and O’Neil, W., editors. Image, Language, Brain. MIT Press, Cambridge MA, pp. 13–14.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ Error – Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. Grosset/Putnam, New York.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1999). The Feeling of What Happens – Body and Emotion in the Making of Consciousness. W. Heinemann, London.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza – Joy, Sorrow, and the Feeling Brain. Vintage, London.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (2006). The Essential Davidson. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Deacon, T.W. (1998). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. Norton, New York.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009) Signatures of consciousness: a talk by Stanislav Dehaene. The Third Culture: Edge in Paris. Available at: . Accessed 1 December 2009.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Kerszberg, M., and Changeux, J.-P. (1998). A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 95:4529–14534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Hertz-Pannier, L., Dubois, J., and Dehaene, S. (2008). How does early brain organization promote language acquisition in humans. European Review 16:405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dennet, D.C. (1995). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. Touchstone, New York.Google Scholar
Einstein, A. (1961). Relativity – The Special and the General Theory. Three Rivers Press, New York.Google Scholar
Erikson, E.H. (1950). Childhood and Society. Norton and Co, New York.Google Scholar
Erikson, E.H. (1977). Toys and Reasons – Stages in the Ritualization of Experience. Norton and Co, New York.Google Scholar
Ferrari, P.F., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., and Fogassi, L. (2003). Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey’s ventral premotor cortex. European Journal of Neuroscience 17:1703–1714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flechsig, P. (1920). Anatomie des menschlichen Gehirns und Rückenmarks. G. Thieme, Leipzig.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1879). Begriffsschrift. Halle, Germany.Google Scholar
Friederici, A. (2002). Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Science 6:78–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuster, J.M. (1997). The Prefrontal Cortex: Anatomy, Physiology, and Neurophsychology of the Frontal Lobe. Lippincott-Raven, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Fuster, J.M. (2002). Physiology of executive functions. In Stuss, D.T., and Knight, M.D., editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK, pp. 96–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuster, J.M. (2003). Cortex and Mind – Unifying Cognition. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK.Google Scholar
Fuster, J.M. (2004). Upper processing stages of the perception action cycle. Trends in Cognitive Science 8:143–145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gage, N.M., and Johnson, M.H. (2007). Development. In Baars, B.J., and Gage, N.M., editors. Cognition, Brain and Consciousness. Academic Press, Burlington MA, pp. 418–419.Google Scholar
Georgiades, Thr. (1982). Music and Language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.Google Scholar
Givón, T. (1995). Functionalism and Grammar. J. Benjamins, Amsterdam/Philadelphia.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grünbaum, A. (1969). The meaning of time. In Rescher, N., editor. Essay’s in Honor of Carl G. Hempel. Reidel, Dordrecht Holland, pp. 147–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatten, M.E. and Heintz, N. (1999). Neurogenesis and migration. In Zigmond, M.J., Bloom, F.E., Landis, S.C., Roberts, J.L., and Squire, L.R., editors. Fundamental Neuroscience. Academic Press, San Diego CA, pp. 451–479.Google Scholar
Hawkins, J. (2005). On Intelligence. Owl Books, New York.Google Scholar
Hebb, D.O. (1949). The Organization of Behaviour – A Neuropsychological Theory. J. Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
Hickok, G., and Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8:393–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubel, D.H., and Wiesel, T.N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction, and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. Journal of Physiology (London) 160:106–154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyengar, B.K.S. (1981). Light on Pranayama. Allen and Unwin, London.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and Cognition. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2002). The Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning, Grammar, Evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R., and Waugh, E. (1979). The Sound Shape of Language. Harvester Press, Brighton Sussex.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. (1963). The Philosophy of Grammar, 9th edition. Allen and Unwin, London.Google Scholar
Joshi, A. (1987). An introduction to tree-adjoining grammars. In Manaster-Ramer, A., editor. Mathematics of Language. J. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 87–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandel, E.R. (1995). Brain and behaviour. In Kandel, E.R., Jessell, J.H., and Schwartz, Th.M., editors. Essentials of Neural Science and Behavior. Prentice Hall, London, pp. 6–19.Google Scholar
Kandel, E.R., Jessell, J.H., and Schwartz, Th.M., editors. (1995). Essentials of Neural Science and Behavior. Prentice Hall, London.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1790). Critique of Teleological Judgement.
Karmiloff, K., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Pathways to Language. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA, p. 12 ff.Google Scholar
Klein, W., and Perdue, C. (1997). The Basic variety, or: couldn’t language be much simpler?Second Language Research 13:301–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G., and Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the Flesh – The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought. Basic Books, New York, p. 19.Google Scholar
Langacker, R.W. (2008). Cognitive Grammar – A Basic Introduction. Oxford University Press, Oxford.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lashley, K. (1950). The search of the engram. Society of Experimental Biology Symposium. 4:454–482.Google Scholar
Le Doux, J. (1996). The Emotional Brain – Mysterious Underpinnings of Emotional Life. Touchstone, New York.Google Scholar
Leibníz, G. (1714). Monadology.
Lichtheim, L. (1885). Über Aphasie. Deutsches Archiv für klinische Medizin 36:204–268.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (2002). Phenomenology of Perception. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London.Google Scholar
Miller, G.A., and Johnson-Laird, Ph.N. (1976). Language and Perception, 2nd edition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montague, R., and Schnelle, H. (1972). Universale Grammatik. Vieweg, Braunschweig.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovitch, M., Chein, J.M., Talmi, D., and Cohn, M. (2007). Learning and memory. In Baars, B.J., and Gage, N.M., editors. Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness. Academic Press, London.Google Scholar
Mountcastle, V.B. (1998). Perceptual Neuroscience – The Cerebral Cortex. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Nauta, W.J.H., and Feirtag, M. (1986). Fundamental Neuroanatomy, Part II. W.H. Freeman, New York.Google Scholar
Penrose, R. (1989). The Emperor’s New Mind – Concerning Computers Minds and the Laws of Physics. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK.Google Scholar
Petrides, M., and Pandya, D.N. (2002). Association pathways of the prefrontal cortex and functional observations. In Stuss, D.T., and Knight, R.T., editors. Principles of the Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK, pp. 31–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1908). Science et Methode. Flammarion, Paris.Google Scholar
Polya, G. (1965). Mathematical Discovery. John Wiley and Sons, New York.Google Scholar
Popper, K.R., and Eccles, J.C. (1977). The Self and its Brain. Springer, Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M.E., and Raichle, M.E. (1997). Images of Mind. Scientific American Library, New York, p. 101.Google Scholar
Powley, T.L. (1999). Central control of autonomous functions – The organization of the autonomic nervous system. In Zigmond, M.J., Bloom, F.E., Landis, S.C., Roberts, J.L., and Squire, L.R., editors. Fundamental Neuroscience. Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 1027–1048.Google Scholar
Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?Behavioural and Brain Science 1:515–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. (2002). The Neuroscience of Language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.Google Scholar
Rafal, R. (2002). Cortical control of visuo-motor reflexes. In Stuss, D.T., and Knight, R.T., editors. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. Oxford University Press, New York, pp. 149–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescher, N. (1991). G.W. Leibniz’s Monadology – An Edition for Students. Routledge, London.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Luppino, G., and Mattelli, M. (1995). Grasping movements: visuomotor transformations. In Arbib, M.A., editor. The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. (2000). Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: a new view on the cortical motor functions. In Gazzaniga, M.S., editor. The New Cognitive Neuroscience. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27:169–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanes, T.H, Reh, T.A., and Harris, W.A. (2006). Development of the Nervous System. 2nd edition. Elsevier Academic Press, Boston.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1978). Poetische Sprache und poetischer Zustand bei Paul Valéry. In Schmidt-Radefeldt, J., editor. Paul Valéry. Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft, Darmstadt, pp. 247–267.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1980a). Wittgenstein on time and tense and the linguistic turn. In Proceedings of the IVth International Wittgenstein Symposium. Hölder – Pichler – Tempski, Wien, pp. 525–531.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1980b). Pretense. In Rohrer, Chr., editor. Time, Tense, and Quantifiers. Niemeyer, Tübingen.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1980c). Introductory remarks on theoretical neurolinguistics. Language Research (Seoul) 16:225–236.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1981a). Phenomenological analysis of language and its application to time and tense. In Parret, H., Sbisà, M., and Verscheuren, J., editors. Possibilities and Limitations of Pragmatics. J. Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp. 631–655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1981b). Introspection and the description of language use. In Coulmas, F., editor. Festschrift for Native Speaker. Mouton, The Hague, pp. 105–126.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1981c). Semantics and pragmatics in psycho-physiological context. In Klein, W., and Levelt, W., editors. Crossing the Boundaries of Linguistics. Reidel, Dordrecht.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1981d). Introduction and Editing; Sprache und Gehirn – In Honor of Roman Jakobson. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1981e). Elements of theoretical net-linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics 8:294–329.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1983). Some preliminary remarks on net-linguistic semantics. In Rickheit, G., and Bock, M., editors. Psycholinguistic Studies in Language Processing. de Gruyter, Berlin, pp. 82–98.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1988). Turing naturalised: Von Neumann’s unfinished project. In Herken, R., editor. The Universal Turing Machine – A Half-Century Survey. Oxford University Press, Oxford UK, pp. 539–557.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (1991). Die Natur der Sprache. de Gruyter, Berlin.Google Scholar
Schnelle, H. (2004). A note on enjoying strawberries with cream, making mistakes, and other idiotic features. In Alan Turing: Life and Legacy of a Great Thinker. Springer, Berlin, pp. 353–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schnelle-Schneyder, M. (1990). Photographie und Wahrnehmung. Jonas Verlag, Marburg.Google Scholar
Schnelle-Schneyder, M. (2003). Sehen und Photographieren. Springer, Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, J.H. (1995). In Kandel, E.R., Jessel, J.H., and Schwartz, Th.M., editors. Essentials of Neural Science and Behavior. Prentice Hall, London, pp. 6–19.Google Scholar
Searle, J.R. (1969). Speech Acts. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J.R. (1983). Intentionality – An Essay in the Philosophy of Mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1992). The Rediscovery of the Mind, 3rd edition. MIT Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1998). Mind, Language and Society. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
Sgall, P. (2006). In Hajicova, E., and Panevova, J., editors. Language in its Multifarious Aspects. The Karolinum Press, Prague.Google Scholar
Singer, W., and Ricard, M. (2008). Hirnforschung und Meditation. Ein Dialog (in German). Suhrkamp, Frankfurt/Main.Google Scholar
Stern, D.N. (1990). Diary of a Baby. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
Stern, D.N. (2000). The Interpersonal World of the Infant, 2nd edition. Basic Books, New York.Google Scholar
Szentagothai, J., and Arbib, M.A. (1975). The module concept in cerebral cortex architecture. Brain Research 95:475–496.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a Language – A Usage Based Theory of Language Acquisition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge MA.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: the Fourteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40A:201–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wachsmuth, I. (2008). ‘I, Max’ – Communicating with an artificial agent. In Wachsmuth, I., and Knoblich, G., editors. Modelling Communication with Robots and Virtual Humans. Springer, Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions Across Languages and Cultures. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wegener, P. (1885). Grundfragen des Sprachenlebens. Niemeyer, Halle. (In German).Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Helmut Schnelle, Ruhr-Universität, Bochum, Germany
  • Book: Language in the Brain
  • Online publication: 05 July 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139193450.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Helmut Schnelle, Ruhr-Universität, Bochum, Germany
  • Book: Language in the Brain
  • Online publication: 05 July 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139193450.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Helmut Schnelle, Ruhr-Universität, Bochum, Germany
  • Book: Language in the Brain
  • Online publication: 05 July 2014
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139193450.012
Available formats
×