Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dzt6s Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-29T17:21:45.516Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Social Contexts and Language Development

Past, Present and Future

from Introduction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2022

James Law
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Sheena Reilly
Affiliation:
Griffith University, Queensland
Cristina McKean
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Get access

Summary

The acquisition of language is a human universal, achieved by all normal children raised in normal environments. However, normal environments vary widely, with consequences for children’s language development. This chapter introduces the subsequent chapters in this edited volume, providing historical background and presenting findings from two contemporary research projects. The historical background includes shifts in the field of language acquisition from linguistics to a bioecological model of development as the guiding theoretical framework, shifts from universals of language acquisition to individual differences as the outcome to be explained, and multiple methodological advances that have made possible much of the research presented in this book. The current empirical work presented in this chapter includes one project on language development in the bilingual context of immigrant families in the United States and another project on home and childcare influences on the language and preliteracy skill development of children in Denmark, in the context of near-universal childcare attendance.

Type
Chapter
Information
Language Development
Individual Differences in a Social Context
, pp. 23 - 40
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bleses, D., Højen, A., Dybdal, L., Dale, P. S., Justice, L. M., Piasta, S., … Aktürk Ari, B. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of a large scale systematic and explicit language and literacy intervention in Danish daycares: The SPELL study. British Journal of Education, Society & Behavioural Science, 4(11), 15041524.Google Scholar
Bleses, D., Højen, A., Justice, L. M., Dale, P. S., Dybdal, L., Piasta, S., … Haghish, E. F. (2018a). The effectiveness of a large-scale language and pre-literacy intervention: The SPELL randomized-controlled-trial in Denmark. Child Development, 89(4), e342e363. doi:10.1111/cdev.12859Google Scholar
Bleses, D., Højen, A., Dale, P. S., Justice, L. M., Dybdal, L., Piasta, S., … Haghish, E. F. (2018b). Effective language and literacy instruction: Evaluating the importance of scripting and group size components. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42, 256269. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleses, D., Makransky, G., Dale, P. S., Højen, A., & Aktürk Ari, B. (2016). Early productive vocabulary predicts academic achievement 10 years later. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(6), 14611476.Google Scholar
Bleses, D., Vach, W., & Wehberg, S. (2008). Individuelle forskelle i danske børns tidlige sprogtilegnelse hvad kan børne: og forældrerelaterede baggrundsfaktorer forklare? (Individual differences in Danish children’s early language addition: What can child and parent-related background factors explain?). Psyche & Logos, 29, 26.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Buac, M., Gross, M., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2014). The role of primary caregiver vocabulary knowledge in the development of bilingual children’s vocabulary skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(5), 18041816.Google Scholar
Burchinal, M., Zaslow, M., & Tarullo, L. (Eds.). (2016). Quality thresholds, features, and dosage in early care and education: Secondary data analyses of child outcomes. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, Vol. 81, Serial No. 321.Google Scholar
Campbell, F. A., Ramey, C. T., Pungello, E., Sparling, J., & Miller-Johnson, S. (2002). Early childhood education: Young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian Project. Applied Developmental Science, 6, 4257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chetty, R., Friedman, J. N., Hilger, N., Saez, E., Schanzenbach, D. W., & Yagan, D. (2011). How does your kindergarten classroom affect your earnings? Evidence from roject STAR. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 126(4), 15931660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T. (2011). Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology and morpho-syntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 318345. doi:10.1075/lab.1.3.05choGoogle Scholar
Corrigan, R. (2012). Using the CHILDES database. In Hoff, E. (Ed.), Research methods in child language: A practical guide (pp. 271284). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Durham, R. E., Farkas, G., Hammer, C. S., Tomblin, J. B., & Catts, H. W. (2007). Kindergarten oral language skill: A key variable in the intergenerational transmission of socioeconomic status. Research in Social Stratification and Mobility, 25, 294305.Google Scholar
Fenson, L., Marchman, V. A., Thal, D., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories: User’s guide and technical manual, 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Foy, J. G., & Mann, V. (2003). Home literacy environment and phonological awareness in preschool children: Differential effects for rhyme and phoneme awareness. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(1), 5988.Google Scholar
Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1979). Language use and language judgment. In Fillmore, C. J., Kempler, D., & Wang, W. S. (Eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (pp. 103126). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263277.Google Scholar
Heath, S. B. (1983). Ways with words: Language, life and work in communities and classrooms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. J., Moon, S. H., Pinto, R., Savelyev, P. A., & Yavitz, A. (2010). The rate of return to the HighScope Perry Preschool Program. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1–2), 114128.Google Scholar
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Owen, M. T., Golinkoff, R. M., Pace, A., … & Suma, K. (2015). The contribution of early communication quality to low-income children’s language success. Psychological Science, 26, 10711083.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Burridge, A., Ribot, K. M., & Giguere, D. (2018). Language specificity in the relation of maternal education to bilingual children’s vocabulary growth. Developmental Psychology, 54(6), 10111019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39, 127.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., Core, C., & Shanks, K. F. (2019). The quality of child-directed speech depends on the speaker’s language proficiency. Journal of Child Language, 47, 132145.Google Scholar
Hoff, E., & Ribot, K. M. (2017). Language growth in English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children from 2.5 to 5 years. The Journal of Pediatrics, 190, 241245.Google Scholar
Højen, A., Bleses, D., Jensen, P., & Dale, P. S. (2019). Patterns of educational achievement among groups of immigrant children in Denmark emerge already in preschool second-language and preliteracy skills. Applied Psycholinguistics, 40(4), 853875. doi:10.1017/S0142716418000814Google Scholar
Højen, A., Hoff, E., Bleses, D., & Dale, P. S. (2021). The relation of home literacy environments to language and preliteracy skills in single- and dual-language children in Danish childcare. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 55, 312325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, J., Waterfall, H., Vasilyeva, M., Vevea, J., & Hedges, L. V. (2010). Sources of variability in children’s language growth. Cognitive Psychology, 61(4), 343365.Google Scholar
Jia, G., Aaronson, D., & Wu, Y. (2002). Long-term language attainment of bilingual immigrants: Predictive variables and language group differences. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 599621. doi:10.1017/S0142716402004058Google Scholar
Justice, L. M., McGinty, A. S., Cabell, S. Q., Kilday, C. R., Knighton, K., & Huffman, G. (2010). Language and literacy curriculum supplement for preschoolers who are academically at risk: A feasibility study. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 161178.Google Scholar
Lauro, J., Core, C., & Hoff, E. (2020). Explaining individual differences in trajectories of simultaneous bilingual development: Contributions of child and environmental factors. Child Development. 91, 20632082.Google Scholar
Love, J. M., Tarullo, L. B., Raikes, H., & Chazan-Cohen, R. (2006). Head Start: What do we know about its effectiveness? What do we need to know? In McCartney, K. & Phillips, D. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of early childhood development (pp. 550575). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melhuish, E., Phan, M. B., Sylva, K., Sammons, P., Siraj-Blatchford, I., & Taggart, B. (2008). Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. In Fletcher-Campbell, F., Soler, J., & Reid, G. (Eds.), Approaching difficulties in literacy development (pp. 95114). Milton Keynes: Open University.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. (2012). Not sampling, getting it all. In Hoff, E. (Ed.), Research methods in child language: A practical guide (pp. 240253). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2002). Child-care structure → process → outcome: Direct and indirect effects of child-care quality on young children’s development. Psychological Science, 13, 199206.Google Scholar
Paradis, J., & Jia, R. (2017). Bilingual children’s long‐term outcomes in English as a second language: Language environment factors shape individual differences in catching up with monolinguals. Developmental Science, 20, e12433.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2011). Properties of dual language exposure that influence 2‐year‐olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 18341849.Google Scholar
Place, S., & Hoff, E. (2016). Effects and noneffects of input in bilingual environments on dual language skills in 2½-year-olds. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 10231041.Google Scholar
Ribot, K. M., & Hoff, E. (2014). ‘¿Cómo estas?’ ‘I’m good.’ Conversational code-switching is related to profiles of expressive and receptive proficiency in Spanish-English bilingual toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38, 333341.Google Scholar
Ribot, K. M., Hoff, E., & Burridge, A. (2018). Language use contributes to expressive language growth: Evidence from bilingual children. Child Development, 89, 929940.Google Scholar
Rowe, M. L. (2012). Recording, transcribing, and coding interaction. In Hoff, E. (Ed.), Research methods in child language: A practical guide (pp. 193207). Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15(1), 163191.Google Scholar
Schweinhart, L. J., Berrueta-Clement, J. R., Barnett, W. S., Epstein, A. S., & Weikart, D. P. (1985). Effects of the Perry Preschool Program on youths through age 19: A summary. Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 5, 2635.Google Scholar
Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. A. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of children’s reading skill: A five‐year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73(2), 445460.Google Scholar
Slot, P. L., Bleses, D., Justice, L. M., Markussen-Brown, J., & Højen, A. (2018). Structural and process quality of Danish preschools: Direct and indirect associations with children’s growth in language and preliteracy skills. Early Education and Development, 29(4), 581602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Votruba‐Drzal, E., & Miller, P. (2016). Reflections on quality and dosage of preschool and children’s development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 81(2), 100113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zauche, L. H., Taylor, A. T., Darcy Mahoney, A. E., & Stapel-Wax, J. L. (2016). Influence of language nutrition on children’s language and cognitive development: An integrated review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 318333.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×