Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T09:57:09.767Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Form–function relations: how do children find out what they are?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2010

Dan I. Slobin
Affiliation:
University of California at Berkeley
Melissa Bowerman
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Psycholinguistik, The Netherlands
Stephen Levinson
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Psycholinguistik, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Human languages, broadly speaking, provide two kinds of meaningful elements, using both to create grammatical constructions. On the one hand, there are morphemes that make reference to the objects and events of experience, and on the other, there are morphemes that relate these bits of experience to each other and to the discourse perspectives of the speaker. Linguistic theories of all stripes honor this duality, using such distinctions as “material content” vs. “relation” (Sapir 1921/1949), “lexical item” vs. “grammatical item” (Lyons 1968), or, most commonly, “content word” vs. “function word” (or, to include both free and bound morphemes, “functor”). Typically, the first class includes nouns and verbs, and usually also adjectives; the second class includes free morphemes such as conjunctions and prepositions, and bound morphemes, such as affixes marking such categories as number, case, tense, and so forth. It has generally been claimed that: (1) functors express a limited and universal set of meanings (“grammaticizable notions”), and (2) functor classes are small and closed, while content word classes are large and open. The first claim has led to proposals that the set of meanings is, in some sense, “prespecified” for language; the second has led to proposals that this collection of morphemes plays a critical role in both the acquisition and processing of language.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×