Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:17:28.714Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Past, Present, and Future Perspectives on Creativity in France and French-Speaking Switzerland

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Christophe Mouchiroud
Affiliation:
Université René Descartes, Paris
Todd Lubart
Affiliation:
Université René Descartes, Paris
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

A summary of past and present research on creativity in France and Romande (French-speaking) Switzerland is proposed in this chapter. This task is difficult, however, because of the cultural diversity of France and Romande Switzerland. Historically, this region has long been a European crossroad for ideas and scientific knowledge. In the field of psychology, only a quick glance at its development reveals the depth and breadth of other cultures' influence, such as the nineteenth-century British empiricist and German experimental approaches, as well as positions of major psychologists such as Freud or James on the nature of the mind. We thus seek to isolate the specific contributions of researchers in France and Romande Switzerland in the understanding of creativity.

We primarily organize our review in a chronological fashion. In the first part, we discuss the writings of early thinkers and researchers, followed by theories exposed at the turn of the nineteenth-century, together with an examination of early empirical contributions. Then, between the two world wars, it seems that studying creativity became less of an issue for psychologists in France and Romande Switzerland, as the field concentrated on more graspable constructs. In the meantime, however, local experiments in the field of education led to the development of various tools and teaching methods that promoted creativity in children. After the Second World War, interest in creativity gained some momentum in France and Romande Switzerland as in many other geographical areas.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anargyros-Klinger, A., Reiss-Schimmel, , & Wainrib, S. (1998). Créations, psychanalyse [Creations, psychoanalysis]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anzieu, D. (1981). Le corps de l'oeuvre [The body of the work]. Paris: GallimardGoogle Scholar
Anzieu, D., Mathieu, M., Besdine, M., Jaques, E., & Guillaumin, J. (Eds.). (1974). Psychanalyse du génie créateur [Psychoanalysis of the creative genius]. Paris: DunodGoogle Scholar
Aznar, G. (1971). Pratique de la créativité dans les entreprises [Creativity practices in business]. Paris: Editions de l'OrganisationGoogle Scholar
Beaudot, A. (1971). Recherches sur la créativité [Research on creativity].Caen, France: Université de CaenGoogle Scholar
Bergson, H. (1919). L'Energie spirituelle [Spiritual energy] (132 ed.). Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceGoogle Scholar
Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. I. (2004, September 11–13). Creative giftedness: Developmental and educational issues. Paper presented at the European Council for High Ability, Pamplona, SpainGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. (1903). Etude expérimentale de l'intelligence. [Experimental study of Intelligence]. Paris: AlcanGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. (1911). Nouvelles recherches sur la mesure du niveau intellectuel chez les enfants des écoles [New research on the measurement of intellectual level in school children]. L'Année Psychologique, 17, 145–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Henri, V. (1895). La psychologie individuelle [Individual psychology]. L'Année Psychologique, 2, 415–465CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux [New methods for the diagnosis of intellectual level in mentally deficient children]. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 191–244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1908). Le développement de l'intelligence chez les enfants. [The development of intelligence in children]. L'Année Psychologique, 14, 1–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnardel, N., & Marmèche, E. (2004). Evocation processes by novice and expert designers: Towards stimulating analogical thinking. Creativity and Innovation Management, 13(3), 176–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braunstein, J.-F., & Pewzner, E. (1999). Histoire de la psychologie [History of psychology]. Paris: ColinGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlier, M. (1973). Etude différentielle d'une modalité de la créativité: La flexibilité.[Differential study of one modality of creativity: flexibility]. Paris: Editions du C.N.R.S.Google Scholar
Carroy, J. (2000). “Mon cerveau est comme dans un crâne de verre”: Emile Zola sujet d'Edouard Toulouse [My brain is like a glass skull: Emile Zola as Edouard Toulouse' subject]. Revue d'histoire du XIXème siècle, 20/21
Centre International de Synthèse (Ed.). (1938). Neuvième semaine internationale de synthèse. L'invention [Ninth international week of synthesis. Invention]. Paris: Alcan
Clandfield, D., & Sivell, J. (Eds.). (1990). Co-operative learning and social change: Selected writings of Celestin Freinet. Toronto: Ontario Institute for Studies in EducationGoogle Scholar
Claparède, E. (1903). L'association des idées [The association of ideas]. Paris: DouinGoogle Scholar
Bono, E. (1992). Serious creativity: Using the power of lateral thinking to create new ideas. New York: HarperCollinsGoogle Scholar
Faucheux, C., & Moscovici, S. (1958). Etudes sur la créativité des groupes: Tâche, situation individuelle et groupe [Studies on group creativity: Task, individual situation, and group]. Bulletin de Psychologie, 152(9)Google Scholar
Faucheux, C., & Moscovici, S. (1960). Etudes sur la créativité des groupes: Tâche, structure des communications et réussite [Studies on group creativity: Task, structure of communications, success]. Bulletin du CERP, 9(1)Google Scholar
Flournoy, T. (1919). Métaphysique et psychologie [Metaphysics and psychology] (2nd ed.). Genève: Kundig. (Original work published 1894)Google Scholar
Flournoy, T., & Claparède, E. (1912). Enquète de “L'enseignement Mathématique” sur la méthode de travail des mathématiciens.[Survey for the review “Mathematics Teaching” on the working method of mathematician]. Paris: Gauthier-Villars Fehr. (Original work pusblished 1902)Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1986). Esquisse d'une psychologie scientifique [Outline of a scientific psychology]. In Bonaparte, M., Freud, A., & Kris, E. (Eds.), La naissance de la psychanalyse. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. (Original work pusblished 1895)Google Scholar
Georgsdottir, A. S., & Lubart, T. I. (2003). La flexibilité cognitive et la créativité: Une approche développementale, différentielle et expérimentale [Cognitive flexibility and creativity: A developmental, differential and experimental approach]. Psychologie Française, 48(3), 29–40Google Scholar
Getz, I., & Robinson, A. G. (2003). Vos idées changent tout [Your ideas change everything]. Paris: Editions d'OrganisationGoogle Scholar
Gordon, W. J. (1956). Operational approach to creativity. Harvard Business Review, 34, 41–51Google Scholar
Guignard, J.-H., & Lubart, T. I. (2004, July 11–15). Empirical study of links between convergent thinking, divergent thinking and childhood giftedness. Paper presented at the Eighteenth Biennial of the International Society for the Study of Behavioural Development, Ghent, BelgiumGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadamard, J. (1945). An essay on the psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. (Published in France in 1975)Google Scholar
Huteau, M. (2003). Psychologie, psychiatry et société sous la troisième république: Edouard Toulouse's Biocracie (1865–1947) [Psychology, psychiatry and society in France's third republic: The Biocracy of Edouard Toulouse]. Paris: L'HarmattanGoogle Scholar
Huteau, M. (in press). La réception de l'Echelle métrique de l'intelligence en France [The acceptance of the metric scale in France]. In Le centenaire de l'échelle métrique de Binet. Paris: L'HarmattanGoogle Scholar
Joly, H. (1882). Psychologie des grands hommes [Psychology of great men]. Revue Philosophique de la France et de l'Étranger, 13, 361–376Google Scholar
Lautrey, J. (1981). L'équilibration suffit-elle à guider la coordination des actions [Does equilibration suffice to guide the coordination of actions]?Psychologie Française, 26, 359–372Google Scholar
Lautrey, J., & De Ribaupierre, A. (2004). Psychology of human intelligence in France and French-speaking Switzerland. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), International handbook of intelligence. Cambridge, U. K.: Cambridge University Press. 104–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lombroso, C. (1889). L'homme de génie [Man of genius]. Paris: AlcanGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I., & Weerd-Nederhof, P. C. (2004). Special Issue-Creativity in the Workplace. Creativity and Innovation Management, 13, 143–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. (1999). Creativity across cultures. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 339–350). Cambridge, U. K.: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I., & Getz, I. (1997). Emotion, metaphor, and the creative process. Creativity Research Journal, 10, 285–301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I., Mouchiroud, C., Tordjman, S., & Zenasni, F. (2003). La psychologie de la créativité [Psychology of creativity]. Paris: Armand ColinGoogle Scholar
Lyons, W. (1986). The disappearance of introspection. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Maury, L. (1988). Freinet et la pédagogie [Freinet and pedagogy]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceGoogle Scholar
Meirieu, P. (Ed.). (2001). Célestin Freinet. Comment susciter le désir d'apprendre [Celestin Freinet: How to arouse the desire for learning]? Paris: PEMFGoogle Scholar
Moreau de Tours, J. J. (1859). La psychology morbide dans ses rapports avec la philosophie de l'histoire, ou de l'influence des névropathies sur le dynamisme intellectuel [Morbid psychology in its relationship with the philosophy of history, or on the influence of neuropathologies on intellectual dynamism]. Paris: MassonGoogle Scholar
Moles, A., & Caude, R. (1970). Créativité et méthodes d'innovation [Creativity and methods of innovation]. Paris: Fayard-MameGoogle Scholar
Mouchiroud, C., & Lubart, T. I. (2001). Children's original thinking: An empirical examination of alternative measures. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 162(4), 382–401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouchiroud, C., & Lubart, T. I. (2002). Social creativity: A cross-sectional study of 6- to 11-year-old children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(1), 60–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicolas, S. (2002). Histoire de la psychology française. Naissance d'une nouvelle science. [History of French psychology. Birth of a new science]. Paris: In Press EditionsGoogle Scholar
Norquay, G. (Ed.) (1999). R. L. Stevenson on fiction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University PressGoogle Scholar
Osborn, A. F. (1953). Applied imagination. New York: Scribner'sGoogle Scholar
Paulhan, F. (1901). Psychologie de l'invention [Psychology of Invention]. Paris: AlcanGoogle Scholar
Piaget, . (1932). Le jugement moral chez l'enfant [Children's moral judgment]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1976). Le possible, l'impossible et le nécessaire (Les recherches en cours ou projetées au Centre International d'Epistémologie Génétique) [The possible, the impossible and the necessary (Research in process or planned at the International Center for Genetic Epistemology)]. Achives de Psychologie, 172, 281–299Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1981). Le possible et le nécessaire chez l'enfant. I. L'évolution des possibles chez l'enfant. [The possible and the necessary in the child. I. The evolution of the possibles in the child]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceGoogle Scholar
Piéron, H. (1903). L'association médiate [Mediate association]. Revue Philosophique de la France et de l'Étranger, 56, 142–149Google Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1908). Science et méthode [Science and method]. Paris: FlammarionGoogle Scholar
Poincaré, H. (1921). The foundations of science. New York: Science PressGoogle Scholar
Raidl, M.-H., & Lubart, T. I. (2000–2001). An empirical study of intuition and creativity. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 20(3), 217–230CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribot, T. (1875). La psychologie anglaise contemporaine [Contemporary English Psychology]. Paris: Germer BaillèreGoogle Scholar
Ribot, T. (1897). L'évolution des idées générales [The evolution of general ideas]. Paris: AlcanGoogle Scholar
Ribot, T. (1900). Essai sur l'imagination créatrice [Essay on creative imagination]. Paris: AlcanGoogle Scholar
Rieben, L. (1978). L'examen des fonctions cognitives chez l'enfant: Intelligence, créativité et opérativité [Examination of cognitive functions in the child: Intelligence, creativity and operativity]. Neuchâtel, Switzerland: Delachaux et NiestléGoogle Scholar
Rieben, L. (1982). Processus secondaires et créativité: Partie émergée de l'iceberg [Secondary processes and creativity: The emerging part of the iceberg]? In Nicolaïdis, N. & Schmid-Kitsikis, E. (Eds.), Créativité et/ou symptôme (pp. 97–111). Paris: Clancier-GuenandGoogle Scholar
Sartre, J. P. (1948). L'imaginaire. Psychologie phénoménologique de l'imagination [The imaginary. Phenomenological psychology of imagination] (11th ed.). Paris: GallimardGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Creativity as blind variation and selective retention: Is the creative process Darwinian?Psychological Inquiry, 10(4), 309–328Google Scholar
Souriau, P. (1881). Théorie de l'invention [Theory of invention]. Paris: HachetteGoogle Scholar
Sudres, J.-L. (2003). Créativité, expression et art-thérapie: Trajectoires …[Creativity, expression and art therapy: Trajectories …]. In Moron, P., Sudres, J.-L., & Roux, G. (Eds.), Créativité et art-thérapie en psychiatrie. Paris: MassonGoogle Scholar
Sully, J. (1876). L'art et la psychologie [Art and Psychology]. Revue Philosophique de la France et de l'Étranger, 2, 321–334Google Scholar
Taine, H. (1870). De l'intelligence [On intelligence]. Paris: HachetteGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1976). Tests de pensée créative [Tests of Creative Thinking]. Paris: Les Editions du Centre de Psychologie AppliquéeGoogle Scholar
Toulouse, E. (1896). Enquête médico-psychologique sur les rapports de la supériorité intellectuelle avec la névropathie. I. Introduction générale. Emile Zola [Medico-psychological study of the relationships between giftedness and neuropathology. General introduction, Emile Zola]. Paris: Société d'Éditions ScientifiquesGoogle Scholar
Toulouse, H. (1910). Enquête médico-psychologique sur la supériorité intellectuelle. II. Henri Poincaré [Medico-psychological study of giftedness. II. Henri Poincaré]. Paris: FlammarionGoogle Scholar
Toulouse, E., & Piéron, H. (1911). Technique de psychologie expérimentale de Toulouse, Vaschide et Piéron.[Toulouse, Uaschide and Piéron's technique of experimental psychology] Paris: DouinGoogle Scholar
Toulouse, E., Vaschide, N., & Piéron, H. (1904). Technique de psychologie expérimentale [Technique of experimental psychology]. Paris: DouinGoogle Scholar
Wallach, M., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. New York: Holt, Rinehart & WinstonGoogle Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt BraceGoogle Scholar
Wolff, C. (1738). Psychologia empirica. Olms; HildesheimGoogle Scholar
Xypas, C. (1997). Piaget et l'éducation [Piaget and Education]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de FranceGoogle Scholar
Zenasni, F., & Lubart, T. I. (2002). Effects of mood states on creativity. Current Psychology Letters, 2(8), 33–50Google Scholar
Zenasni, F., Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. I. (2004). A developmental study of tolerance of ambiguity and its links with creativity. Manuscript submitted for publication

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×