Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T05:45:26.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Creativity in Soviet–Russian Psychology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Olga Stepanossova
Affiliation:
Moscow State University/Center for the Psychology of Abilities, Competencies, and Expertise, Yale University
Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Moscow State University/Center for the Psychology of Abilities, Competencies, and Expertise, Yale University
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
California State University, San Bernardino
Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

In this chapter we present an overview of creativity in Soviet–Russian psychology. We are using the term Soviet–Russian psychology to indicate that the content of this chapter is based primarily on studies and conceptions developed in the context of Soviet psychology prior to December 1991, when the Soviet Union was disassembled, as well as on studies and theories that have appeared more recently in Russia. This chapter shows an existing mosaic of approaches to creativity in Soviet–Russian psychology. We present original conceptual and methodological solutions to questions on the nature of creativity and ways to study, and we discuss similarities and common characteristics of these solutions with Western psychological tradition.

With all its richness and diversity, the field of Soviet–Russian psychology of creativity can be understood as reflecting two major trajectories. The first trajectory can be traced back to conceptualizations of insight and creative process in Gestalt psychology (Duncker, 1945; Wertheimer, 1959). Soviet–Russian psychologists, attempting to understand this work, reinterpreted and reconceptualized creative process by implementing general principals of Marxist psychology developed, by that time, in the area of the psychology of thinking (see Grigorenko, Ruzgis, & Sternberg, 1997). The intention was to demystify insight by finding its concrete psychological mechanisms and the objective environmental conditions that lead to its occurrence, and, further, to explain creative processes from deterministic positions.

The second trajectory can be traced to the acquaintance of Soviet– Russian psychological thought with Guilford's view of creativity and with the Guilford and Torrance tests of creativity.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. M., Goldfarb, P., & Brackfield, S. C. (1990). Social influences on creativity: Evaluation, coaction, and surveillance. Creativity Research Journal, 3, 6–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Babaeva, Y. D. (1997). Dinamicheskaya teoriya odarennosti [The dynamic theory of giftedness]. In Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (Ed.), Osnovnye sovremennye kontseptsii tvorchestva i odarennosti (pp. 275–294). Moscow: Molodaya GvardiyaGoogle Scholar
Bakhtin, M. (1984). Problems of Dostoevsky's poetics. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bibler, V. S. (1983). Thinking as creation: Introduction to the logic of mental dialogue. Soviet Psychology, 22, 33–54Google Scholar
Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (1983). Intellectual'naya aktivnost' kak problema tvorchestva [Intellectual activity as a problem of creativity]. Rostov-na-Donu, Russia: Rostovskii Gosudarstvennyi UniversitetGoogle Scholar
Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (1995). O predmete i metode issledovaniya tvorcheskikh sposobnostei [About the subject and method of studying creative abilities]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 16, 49–58Google Scholar
Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (1999). Sub'yekt deyatel'nosti ⅴ problematike tvorchestva [“Activity subject” in creativity context]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 2, 35–41Google Scholar
Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (2002). Psikhologiya tvorcheskikh sposobnostey [The psychology of creative abilities]. Moscow: AkademiyaGoogle Scholar
Brushlinsky, A. V. (1996). Sub'yekt: Myshleniye, ucheniye, voobrazheniye [Individual: Thinking, learning, imagination]. Voronezh, Russia: Institut Prakticheskoy PsikhologiiGoogle Scholar
Brushlinsky, A. V. (1997). O sub'yekte myshleniya i tvorchestva [About the subject of thinking and creativity]. In Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (Ed.), Osnovnye sovremennye kontseptsii tvorchestva i odarennosti (pp. 39–56). Moscow: Molodaya GvardiyaGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Harper & RowGoogle Scholar
Druzhinin, V. N. (1997). Struktura psikhometricheskogo intellekta i prognoz individual'nykh dostizhenii [The structure of psychometric intelligence and prediction of individual achievements]. In Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (Ed.), Osnovnye sovremennye kontseptsii tvorchestva i odarennosti (pp. 161–185). Moscow: Molodaya GvardiyaGoogle Scholar
Druzhinin, V. N. (1999). Psikhologiya obschikh sposobnostei [Psychology of general abilities]. Saint Petersburg, Russia: PiterGoogle Scholar
Druzhinin, V. N., & Khazratova, N. V. (1994). Eksperimental'noe issledovanie formiruyuschego vliyaniya mikrosredy na kreativnost' [An experimental study of the formative influence of microenvironments on creativity]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 15, 83–93Google Scholar
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 58, 1–113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, S., Pacini, R., Denes-Raj, V., & Heier, H. (1996). Individual differences in intuitive – experiential and analytical – rational thinking styles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 390–405CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frantz, R. (2003). Herbert Simon. Artificial intelligence as a framework for understanding intuition. Journal of Economic Psychology, 24, 265–277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galperin, P. Y. (1969). Stages in the development of mental acts. In Cole, M., & Maltzman, I. (Eds.), A handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology (pp. 34–61). New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Galperin, P. Y. (1989). Mental actions as a basis for the formation of thoughts and images. Soviet Psychology, 27, 45–64Google Scholar
Galperin, P. Y., & Kotik, N. R. (1982). K psikhologii tvorcheskogo myshleniya [On the psychology of creative thinking]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 5, 80–84Google Scholar
Goryunova, N. B., & Druzhinin, V. N. (2001). Operatsional'nye deskriptory kognitivnogo resursa i productivnost' resheniya testovykh zadach i zadach-golovolomok [Operational descriptors of the cognitive resource and productivity of test tasks and puzzle solving]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 22, 13–22Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., & Kornilova, T. V. (1997). The resolution of the nature–nurture controversy by Russian psychology: Culturally biased or culturally specific? In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), Intelligence, heredity, and environment (pp. 393–439). New York: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Ruzgis, P., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.). (1997). Psychology of Russia: Past, present, future. Commack, NY: Nova ScienceGoogle Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
De Groot, A. D. (1993). Intuition as a dispositional concept. In Heck, G. L., Bonaiuto, P., Deary, I. J., & Nowack, W. (Eds.), Personality psychology in Europe (Vol. 4, pp. 7–50). Tilburg, Netherlands: Tilburg University PressGoogle Scholar
Hammond, K. R. (2000). Judgments under stress. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Heller, K. A., Gaedike, A. K., & Weinlader, H. (1985). Kognitiven Fahigkeitstest (KFT4–13+) [Test of Cognitive Capabilities (KFT4–13+)]. Weinheim, Germany: BeltzGoogle Scholar
Hogarth, R. M. (2001). Educating intuition. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the study of statistical intuitions. Cognition, 11, 123–141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanevskaya, M. E., & Firsova, L. A. (1990). Svyaz' uspeshnosti khudozhestvennogo tvorchestva s napravlennost'yu lichnosti u detei [Correlation between successfulness of artistic creativity and personality trends in children]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 3, 69–75Google Scholar
Kholodnaya, M. A. (2001). Psikhologiya intellekta: Paradoksy issledovaniya [Psychology of intelligence: Research paradoxes]. Saint Petersburg, Russia: PiterGoogle Scholar
Kholodnaya, M. A. (2002). Osnovnye napravleniya izucheniya psykhologii sposobnostei ⅴ institute psikhologii RAN [Major approaches to studying psychological abilities elaborated in the Institute of Psychology, RAS]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 23, 13–22Google Scholar
Krogius, N. V. (1981). Psikhologiya shakhmatnogo tvorchestva [Psychology of chess creativity]. Moscow: Fizkultura SportGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. A. (1998). “Nauchite cheloveka fantazii …” (tvorchestvo i razvivayuschee obrazovanie) [“Teach a fantasy …” (Creativity and developing education)]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 5, 82–85Google Scholar
Leontiev, A. N. (1978a). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-HallGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. N. (1978b). Thinking. In Great Soviet Encyclopedia (3rd ed., trans., pp. 708–709). New York: Macmillan Educational CorporationGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. N., Ponomarev, Y. A., & Gippenreiter, Y. B. (1981). Opyt eksperimental'nogo issledovaniya myshleniya [An experience of an experimental study of thinking]. In Gippenreiter, Y. D. & Petukhov, V. V. (Eds.), Khrestomatiya po obschey psikhologii: Psikhologiya myshleniya (pp. 269–280). Moscow: Moskovskii Gosudarstvennyi UniversitetGoogle Scholar
Lubart, T. I. (2001). Models of the creative process: Past, present and future. Creativity Research Journal, 13, 295–308CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matyushkin, A. M. (1989). Kontseptsiya tvorcheskoi odarennosti [The conception of creative giftedness]. Voprosy Psykhologii, 6, 29–33Google Scholar
Matyushkin, A. M. (1990). A Soviet perspective on giftedness and creativity. European Journal for High Ability, 1, 72–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matyushkin, A. M. (1993). Zagadki odarennosti: Problemy prakticheskoi diagnostiki [The puzzles of giftedness: The issues of practical diagnostics.] Moscow: Shkola-PressGoogle Scholar
Mednick, S. A., & Mednick, M. T. (1967). Examiner's manual: Remote Associates Test.Boston: Houghton MifflinGoogle Scholar
Obukhova, L. F., & Churbanova, S. M. (1994). Razvitiye divergentnogo myshleniya ⅴ detskom vozraste [Development of divergent thinking in childhood]. Moscow: Moscovskii Gosudarstvennyi UniversitetGoogle Scholar
Ozhiganova, G. V. (2001). Diagnostika i formirovanie kreativnosti u detey ⅴ protsesse uchebnoi deyatel'nosti [Diagnostics and forming of creativity in children in the process of school learning]. Psikhologicheskii Zhurnal, 22, 75–85Google Scholar
Petukhov, V. V. (1999). Problema osmyslennogo deistviya (po resheniyu tvorcheskikh zadach) [The problem of intelligent action (in creative problem solving)]. In Voiskunsky, A. E., Zhdan, A. N., & Tikhomirov, O. K., Traditsii i perspektivy deyatel'nostnogo podkhoda ⅴ psikhologii: Shkola Leontieva (pp. 235–262). Moscow: SmyslGoogle Scholar
Ponomarev, I. A. (1960). Psikhologiya tvorcheskogo myshleniya [The psychology of creative thinking]. Moscow: Academiya Pedagogicheskikh Nauk RSFSRGoogle Scholar
Ponomarev, I. A. (1987). Osnovnye zven'ya psikhologicheskogo mekhanizma tvorchestva [The major components of psychological mechanism of creativity]. In Panov, M. I. (Ed.), Intuitsiya, logika, tvorchestvo (pp. 5–23). Moscow: NaukaGoogle Scholar
Reber, A. S., Walkenfeld, F. F., & Hernstadt, R. (1991). Implicit and explicit learning: Individual differences and IQ. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 1, 888–896Google Scholar
Ryabova, T. V., & Mendelevich, V. D. (2002). Tvorcheskoe myshlenie i antitsipatsiya u bol'nyh shizofreniey i psikhicheski zdorovikh [Creative thinking and anticipation in schizophrenic patients and mentally healthy people]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 1, 69–75Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1989). Making management decisions: The role of intuition and emotion. In Agor, W. H. (Ed.), Intuition in organizations: Leading and managing productively (pp. 23–39). Newbury Park, CA: SageGoogle Scholar
Shirley, D. A., & Lagan-Fox, J. (1996). Intuition: A review of the literature. Psychological Reports, 79, 563–584CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shcheblanova, E. I. (1999). Osobennosti kognitivnogo i motivatsionno-lichnostnogo razvitiya odarennykh starsheklassnikov [Characteristics of cognitive and motivational-personality development of gifted high school students]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 6, 36–47Google Scholar
Shcheblanova, E. I. (2000). The dynamics of cognitive and noncognitive personality indicators of giftedness in younger schoolchildren. Russian Education and Society, 42, 5–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shumakova, N. B. (1996). Mezhdistsiplinarnyi podkhod k obucheniyu odarennykh detei. [Interdisciplinary approach to the teaching of gifted children]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 3, 34–43Google Scholar
Shumakova, N. B., Shcheblanova, G. I., & Shcherbo, N. P. (1991). Issledovanie odarennosti s ispol'zovaniem testov P. Torrensa [The usage of P. Torrance's Tests in study of giftedness]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 1, 27–32Google Scholar
Tikhomirov, O. K. (1975). Psikhologicheskie issledovaniya tvorcheskoy deyatel'nosti [Psychological studies of creative activity]. Moscow: NaukaGoogle Scholar
Tikhomirov, O. K., Babayeva, Y. D., Berezanskaya, N. B., Vasilyev, I. A., & Voyskunsky, A. E. (1999). Razvitie deyatel'nostnogo podkhoda ⅴ psikhologii myshleniya [Development of the activity approach to the psychology of thinking]. In Voiskunsky, A. E., Zhdan, A. N., & Tikhomirov, O. K., Traditsii i perspektivy deyatel'nostnogo podkhoda ⅴ psikhologii: Shkola Leontieva (pp. 191–234). Moscow: SmyslGoogle Scholar
Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-HallCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treffinger, D. J. (1980). Encouraging creative learning for the gifted and talented. A handbook of methods and techniques.Ventura, CA: Ventura County Superintendent of Schools OfficeGoogle Scholar
Ushakov, D. V. (1988). Rol' metafory ⅴ tvorcheskom myshlenii [The role of metaphor in creative thinking]. Vestnik Vysshey Shkoly, 1, 24–28Google Scholar
Ushakov, D. V. (1997). Odarennost', tvorchestvo, intuitsiya [Giftedness, creativity, intuition]. In Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (Ed.), Osnovnye sovremennye kontseptsii tvorchestva i odarennosti (pp. 78–89). Moscow: Molodaya GvardiyaGoogle Scholar
Ushakov, D. V. (1999). Myshleniye i intellekt [Thinking and intelligence]. In Druzhinin, V. N. (Ed.), Sovremennaya psikhologiya: Spravochnoye rukovodstvo (pp. 241–266). Moscow: Infra-MGoogle Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallas, G. (1926). The art of thought. New York: McGraw-HillGoogle Scholar
Wertheimer, M. (1959). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & RowGoogle Scholar
Yaroshevsky, M. G. (1985). Psikhologiya tvorchestva i tvorchestvo ⅴ psikhologii [Psychology of creativity and creative work in psychology]. Voprosy Psikhologii, 6, 14–26Google Scholar
Yurkevich, V. S. (1996). Odarennyi rebenok: Illyuzii i real'nost' [A gifted child: Illusions and reality]. Moscow: ProsveshchenieGoogle Scholar
Yurkevich, V. S. (1997). O “naivnoi” i “kul'turnoi” kreativnosti [About naïve and cultural creativity]. In Bogoyavlenskaya, D. B. (Ed.), Osnovnye sovremennye kontseptsii tvorchestva i odarennosti (pp. 127–142). Moscow: Molodaya GvardiyaGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×