Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T19:17:59.592Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Before You Dive In

Foundational Issues in Social Science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2024

Harry T. Reis
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Tessa West
Affiliation:
New York University
Charles M. Judd
Affiliation:
University of Colorado Boulder
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Abele-Brehm, A. E., Gollwitzer, M., Steinberg, U., and Schönbrodt, F. D. (2019). Attitudes toward open science and public data sharing: A survey among members of the German Psychological Society. Social Psychology, 50(4), 252260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aly, M., Colunga, E., Crockett, M. J., Goldrick, M., Gomez, P., Kung, F. Y. H., McKee, P. C., Pérez, M., Stilwell, S. M., and Diekman, A. B. (2022). Changing the culture of peer review for a more inclusive and equitable psychological science. Preprint available at https://psyarxiv.com/435xz.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (1994) Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2001) Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2010) Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, 6th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association (2017) Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct. Available from the author at www.apa.org/ethics/code.Google Scholar
Azar, M., Riehm, K. E., McKay, D., and Thombs, B. D. (2015). Transparency of outcome reporting and trial registration of randomized controlled trials published in the Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. PLOS ONE, 10(11), e0142894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azpeitia, J., Lombard, E. J., Pope, T., and Cheryan, S. (2022). Reference Audit: A Technique for Diversifying Your References. Seattle, WA: University of Washington (citation audit template available at https://docs.google.com/spreadsheets/d/1HHM6i1WDaAVzSJxgZG8UtNWP3aRcFprsdVpLZJEWxOQ/edit#gid=759055580).Google Scholar
Bakker, M., Veldkamp, C. L., van Assen, M. A., Crompvoets, E. A., Ong, H. H., Nosek, B. A., Soderberg, C. K., Mellor, D., & Wicherts, J. M. (2020). Ensuring the quality and specificity of preregistrations. PLoS biology, 18(12), e3000937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, P. J. (2022). Psychological science stepping up a level. Psychological Science, 33(2), 179183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics of research: After reading Milgram’s “behavioral study of obedience.American Psychologist, 19(6), 421423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bem, D. (2003). Writing the empirical journal article. In Darley, J. M., Zanna, M. P., and Roediger III, H. L. (eds.) The Compleat Academic: A Practical Guide for the Beginning Social Scientist, 2nd ed. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bem, D. (2011). Feeling the future: Experimental evidence for anomalous retroactive influences on cognition and affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 407425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamin, D. J., Berger, J. O., Johannesson, M., Nosek, B. A., Wagenmakers, E. J., Berk, R., … and Johnson, V. E. (2018). Redefine statistical significance. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(1), 610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biros, M. (2018). Capacity, vulnerability, and informed consent for research. Journal of Law and Medical Ethics, 46, 7278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boynton, M. H., Portnoy, D. B., and Johnson, B. T. (2013). Exploring the ethics and psychological impact of deception in psychological research. IRB, 35(2), 713.Google ScholarPubMed
Chambers, C. D., and Tzavella, L. (2022). The past, present and future of registered reports. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(1), 2942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, L. (1988). Deception in psychological research: When is its use justified? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14(4), 664675.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christian, T. M. L., Lafferty-Hess, S., Jacoby, W. G., and Carsey, T. (2018). Operationalizing the replication standard. International Journal of Digital Curation, 13(1), 114124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christie, D. J. (2011). The Encyclopedia of Peace Psychology. John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claesen, A., Gomes, S., Tuerlinckx, F., and Vanpaemel, W. (2021). Comparing dream to reality: An assessment of adherence of the first generation of preregistered studies. Royal Society Open Science, 8(10), 211037.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coffelt, T. A. (2017). Confidentiality and anonymity of participants. In Allen, M. (ed.), The SAGE Encyclopedia of Communication Research Methods, SAGE publications.Google Scholar
Constine, J. (2014). The morality of A/B testing. TechCrunch+. At https://techcrunch.com/2014/06/29/ethics-in-a-data-driven-world.Google Scholar
Craig, R., Cox, A., Tourish, D., and Thorpe, A. (2020). Using retracted journal articles in psychology to understand research misconduct in the social sciences: What is to be done? Research Policy, 49(4), 103930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandall, C. S. (2001). Scientific progress: A need for trust, a need for skepticism. Dialogue, 16(2), 2021.Google Scholar
Crandall, C. S. (2019). Science as dissent: The practical value of basic and applied science. Journal of Social Issues, 75(2), 630641.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Criado-Perez, C. (2021). Measuring and predicting the use of evidence-based management. Ph.D. dissertation, UNSW Sydney.Google Scholar
Crosas, M., Gautier, J., Karcher, S., Kirilova, D., Otalora, G., and Schwartz, A. (2018). Data policies of highly-ranked social science journals. At https://osf.io/preprints/socarxiv/9h7ay.Google Scholar
Curtis, B. L. (2014). Social networking and online recruiting for HIV research: Ethical challenges. Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, 9, 5870.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devezer, B., Navarro, D. J., Vandekerckhove, J., and Ozge Buzbas, E. (2020). The case for formal methodology in scientific reform. Royal Society Open Science, 8(3), 200805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebersole, C. R., Atherton, O. E., Belanger, A. L., Skulborstad, H. M., Allen, J. M., Banks, J. B., … and Nosek, B. A. (2016). Many labs 3: Evaluating participant pool quality across the academic semester via replication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 6882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engber, D. (2017) Daryl Bem proved ESP is real: Which means science is broken. Slate, June 7, 2017.Google Scholar
Fanelli, D. (2009). How many scientists fabricate and falsify research? A systematic review and meta-analysis of survey data. PLOS ONE, 4(5), e5738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, N., Honeycutt, N., and Jussim, L. (2022). Better understanding the population size and stigmatization of psychologists using questionable research practices. Meta-psychology, 6, MP.2020.2601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilmore, R., and Nilsonne, G. (2017). IRBs and data sharing. At https://gilmore-lab.github.io/sips-2017-07-30/#5.Google Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R. (2012). Science or art? How aesthetic standards grease the way through the publication bottleneck but undermine science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 562571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Approaching a fair deal for significance and other concerns. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 65, 16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giofrè, D., Boedker, I., Cumming, G., Rivella, C., and Tressoldi, P. (March 7, 2022). The influence of journal submission guidelines on authors’ reporting of statistics and use of open research practices: Five years later. MetaArXiv. At https://doi.org/10.31222/osf.io/8ya3m.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzales, J. E., and Cunningham, C. A. (2015). The promise of preregistration in psychological research. Psychological Science Agenda, 29(8), 2014–2017.Google Scholar
Gopalakrishna, G., ter Riet, G., Vink, G., Stoop, I., Wicherts, J. M., and Bouter, L. M. (2022). Prevalence of questionable research practices, research misconduct and their potential explanatory factors: A survey among academic researchers in the Netherlands. PLOS ONE, 17(2), e0263023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. H. (1948). Ethics governing the service of prisoners as subjects in medical experiments. Journal of the American Medical Association, 136, 457458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G. (1975). Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 82(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunsalus, C. K., Marcus, A. R., and Oransky, I. (2018). Institutional research misconduct reports need more credibility. JAMA, 319(13), 13151316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gupta, U. C. (2013). Informed consent in clinical research: Revisiting few concepts and areas. Perspectives in Clinical Research, 4(1), 2632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hailes, H. P., Ceccolini, C. J., Gutowski, E., and Liang, B. (2021). Ethical guidelines for social justice in psychology. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 52(1), 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardwicke, T. E., Bohn, M., MacDonald, K., Hembacher, E., Nuijten, M. B., Peloquin, B. N., … and Frank, M. C. (2021). Analytic reproducibility in articles receiving open data badges at the journal Psychological Science: An observational study. Royal Society Open Science, 8(1), 201494, DOI:10.1098/rsos.201494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardwicke, T. E., and Ioannidis, J. P. (2018). Populating the data ark: An attempt to retrieve, preserve, and liberate data from the most highly-cited psychology and psychiatry articles. PLOS one, 13(8), e0201856, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0201856.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardwicke, T. E., Mathur, M. B., MacDonald, K., Nilsonne, G., Banks, G. C., Kidwell, M. C., … Frank, M. C. (2018). Data availability, reusability, and analytic reproducibility: Evaluating the impact of a mandatory open data policy at the journal cognition. Royal Society Open Science, 5(8), 180448, DOI: 10.1098/rsos.180448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardwicke, T. E., Thibault, R. T., Kosie, J. E., Wallach, J. D., Kidwell, M. C., and Ioannidis, J. P. (2022). Estimating the prevalence of transparency and reproducibility-related research practices in psychology (2014–2017). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(1), 239251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heirene, R., LaPlante, D., Louderback, E. R., Keen, B., Bakker, M., Serafimovska, A., and Gainsbury, S. M. (2021, July 16). Preregistration specificity & adherence: A review of preregistered gambling studies & cross-disciplinary comparison. At https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/nj4es.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houtkoop, B. L., Chambers, C., Macleod, M., Bishop, D. V., Nichols, T. E., and Wagenmakers, E. J. (2018). Data sharing in psychology: A survey on barriers and preconditions. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(1), 7085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivy, A. C. (1949). Nazi war crimes of a medical nature. Journal of the American Medical Association, 139, 131135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacoby, W. (March 26, 2015). The AJPS replication policy: Innovations and revisions. American Journal of Political Science Editor Blog. At https://ajps.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/03/ajps-replic-and-verif-policy-2-27-18.pdf.Google Scholar
Jin, X., Chandramouli, C., Allocco, B., Gong, E., Lam, C. S. P., and Yan, L. L. (2020). Women’s participation in cardiovascular clinical trials from 2010–2017. Circulation, 141, 540548.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, K. J., and Cesario, J. (2016). How can preregistration contribute to research in our field? Comprehensive Results in Social Psychology, 1(1–3), 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, C. H., and Zanna, M. P. (1999). How to read a journal article in social psychology. In Baumeister, R. F. (ed.) The Self in Social Psychology. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kerr, N. L. (1998). HARKing: Hypothesizing after the results are known. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(3), 196217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kidwell, M. C., Lazarević, L. B., Baranski, E., Hardwicke, T. E., Piechowski, S., Falkenberg, L. S., … and Nosek, B. A. (2016). Badges to acknowledge open practices: A simple, low-cost, effective method for increasing transparency. PLoS Biology, 14(5), e1002456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kisselburgh, L., and Beever, J. (2022). The ethics of privacy in research and design: Principles, practices, and potential. In Knijnenburg, B. P., Page, X., Wisniewski, P., Lipford, H. R., Proferes, N., and Romano, J. (eds.) Modern Socio-technical Perspectives on Privacy. Springer.Google Scholar
Klein, R. A., Vianello, M., Hasselman, F., Adams, B. G., AdamsJr, R. Alper, B., S., … and Sowden, W. (2018). Many labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across samples and settings. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 443490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2014). What can recent replication failures tell us about the theoretical commitments of psychology? Theory & Psychology, 24, 326338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korn, J. H. (1997). Illusions of Reality: A History of Deception in Social Psychology. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Lawson, K. M., Murphy, B., Azpeitia, J., Lombard, E. Pope, T., and Hall, J. A. (2022). Citing behaviors in psychology: A roadblock to cumulative and inclusive science (preprint). Available at https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/6kvqg.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, M. M., and Harbin, J. J. (1997). Psychological ethics codes: A comparison of twenty‐four countries. International Journal of Psychology, 32(3), 181192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBel, E. P., Borsboom, D., Giner-Sorolla, R., Hasselman, F., Peters, K. R., Ratliff, K. A., and Smith, C. T. (2013). PsychDisclosure. org: Grassroots support for reforming reporting standards in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 424432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeBel, E. P., and Peters, K. R. (2012). Fearing the future of empirical psychology: Bem’s (2011) evidence of psi as a case study of deficiencies in modal research practice. Review of General Psychology, 15(4), 371379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledgerwood, A. (2018). The preregistration revolution needs to distinguish between predictions and analyses. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(45), E10516E10517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Texier, T. (2019). Debunking the Stanford prison experiment. American Psychologist, 74, 823839.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longino, H. E. (2020). Science as social knowledge. In Longino, H. E., Science as Social Knowledge. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maassen, E., Van Assen, M. A., Nuijten, M. B., Olsson-Collentine, A., and Wicherts, J. M. (2020). Reproducibility of individual effect sizes in meta-analyses in psychology. PLOS one, 15(5), e0233107, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDaniel, T., and Starmer, C. (1998). Experimental economics and deception: A comment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 19, 403409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majeed, A., and Lee, S. (2020). Anonymization techniques for privacy preserving data publishing: A comprehensive survey. IEEE Access, 9, 85128545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martone, M. E., Garcia-Castro, A., and VandenBos, G. R. (2018). Data sharing in psychology. American Psychologist, 73(2), 111125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayo, D. G. (2018). Statistical Inference as Severe Testing. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merton, R. K. (1942). The normative structure of science. In Merton, R. K. (ed.) The Sociology of Science: Theoretical and Empirical Investigations. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meyer, M. N. (2018). Practical tips for ethical data sharing. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1, 131144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michelzon, T. (2021). Dan Ariely was suspended from research at MIT after conducting unauthorized experiment with human subjects. HaMakom. At www.ha-makom.co.il/post-tomer-dan-ariely-mit-suspention.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitscherlich, A., and Mielke, F. (1949). Doctors of Infamy: The Story of the Nazi Medical Crimes. Henry Schuman.Google Scholar
Murayama, K., Pekrun, R., and Fiedler, K. (2014). Research practices that can prevent an inflation of false-positive rates. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(2), 107118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narayanan, A., and Shmatikov, V. (2008, May). Robust de-anonymization of large sparse datasets. In 2008 IEEE Symposium on Security and Privacy (sp 2008). IEEE.Google Scholar
National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research; Department of Health, Education and Welfare (September 30, 1978). The Belmont Report. United States Government Printing Office. DHEW pub. no. (OS) 78-0012. At http://videocast.nih.gov/pdf/ohrp_belmont_report.pdf.Google Scholar
National Research Council. (2014). Proposed Revisions to the Common Rule for the Protection of Human Subjects in the Behavioral and Social Sciences. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Ndebele, P. (2013). The Declaration of Helsinki, 50 years later. Jama, 310(20), 21452146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., … and Yarkoni, T. (2015). Promoting an open research culture: The TOP guidelines for journals. Science, 348(6242), 14221425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., and Lakens, D. (2014). Registered reports: A method to increase the credibility of published results. Social Psychology, 45(3), 137141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuijten, M. B., Borghuis, J., Veldkamp, C. L. S., Dominguez-Alvarez, L., van Assen, M. A. L. M., and Wicherts, J. M. (2017). Journal data sharing policies and statistical reporting inconsistencies in psychology. Collabra: Psychology, 3(1), doi.org/10.1525/collabra.102.Google Scholar
Code, The Nuremberg. (1947). In A. Mitscherlich and F. Mielke, Doctors of Infamy: The Story of the Nazi Medical Crimes. New York: Schuman (1949), xxiiixxv.Google Scholar
Obels, P., Lakens, D., Coles, N. A., Gottfried, J., and Green, S. A. (2020). Analysis of open data and computational reproducibility in registered reports in psychology. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(2), 229237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ojanuga, D. (1993). The medical ethics of the “father of gynecology,” Dr J Marion Sims. Journal of Medical Ethics, 19(1), 2831.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oransky, I., and Marcus, N. (2016). How researchers lock up their study data with sharing fees. STAT. At www.statnews.com/2016/09/30/data-sharing-fees.Google Scholar
Pace, L. A., and Livingston, M. M. (2005). Protecting human subjects in internet research. Electronic Journal of Business Ethics and Organization Studies, 10, 3540.Google Scholar
Painter, A., and Sterba, S. (eds.) (2011). Handbook of Ethics in Quantitative Methodology. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Singh, T., and Scoboria, A. (2010). Using deception ethically: Practical research guidelines for researchers and reviewers. Canadian Psychology, 51(4), 241248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Painter, A., and Sterba, S. (eds.) (2011). Handbook of Ethics in Quantitative Methodology. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rea, S. C., Kleeman, H., Zhu, Q., Gilbert, B., and Yue, C. (2020). Crowdsourcing as a tool for research: Methodological, fair, and political considerations. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 40(3–4), 4053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reverby, S. M. (ed.). (2012). Tuskegee’s Truths: Rethinking the Tuskegee Syphilis Study. UNC Press Books.Google Scholar
Riehm, K. E., Azar, M., and Thombs, B. D. (2015). Transparency of outcome reporting and trial registration of randomized controlled trials in top psychosomatic and behavioral health journals: A 5-year follow-up. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 79(1), 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rochios, C., and Richmond, J. L. (2022). Are we all on the same page? Subfield differences in open science practices in psychology. Infant and Child Development, 33(1), e2361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosnow, R. L., and Rosenthal, R. (2011). Ethical principles in data analysis: An overview. In Painter, A. and Sterba, S. (eds.) Handbook of Ethics in Quantitative Methodology. Routledge.Google Scholar
Rubin, Mark. (2020). Does preregistration improve the credibility of research findings? Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 16(4), 376390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakaluk, J., Williams, A., and Biernat, M. (2014). Analytic review as a solution to the misreporting of statistical results in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 652660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheel, A. M. (2022). Why most psychological research findings are not even wrong. Infant and Child Development, 31(1), e2295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scheel, A. M., Schijen, M. R., and Lakens, D. (2021). An excess of positive results: Comparing the standard psychology literature with registered reports. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 4(2), 25152459211007467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Settles, I. H., Jones, M. K., Buchanan, N. T., and Dotson, K. (2021). Epistemic exclusion: Scholar(ly) devaluation that marginalizes faculty of color. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 14(4), 493507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R., and Nesbitt, R. (2017). Don’t let your misunderstanding of the rules hinder your research. Chronicle of Higher Education. Retrieved from http://chronicle.com.Google Scholar
Shuster, E. (1997). Fifty years later: The significance of the Nuremberg Code. New England Journal of Medicine, 337(20), 14361440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., and Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 13591366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
SPSP TOP II Task Force (2022). Resources: TOP II guidelines, FAQ/guidance. Retrieved from https://spsp.org/professional-development/publishing-resources/top-ii-guidelines-faq-guidance#1.Google Scholar
Stern, P. C. (2011). Contributions of psychology to limiting climate change. American Psychologist, 66(4), 303314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. S., and Sternberg, K. (2010). The Psychologist’s Companion: A Guide to Scientific Writing for Students and Researchers, 2nd ed. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, D. H., White, S. A., and Bierer, B. E. (2021). Justice, diversity, and research ethics review. Science, 371(6535), 12091211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stricker, J., and Günther, A. (2019). Scientific misconduct in psychology: A systematic review of prevalence estimates and new empirical data. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 227(1), 5363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweeney, L. (2000). Simple demographics often identify people uniquely. Health, 671(2000), 134.Google Scholar
Tedersoo, L., Küngas, R., Oras, E. et al. Data sharing practices and data availability upon request differ across scientific disciplines. Sci Data 8(192) (2021), https://doi.org/10.1038/s41597-021-00981-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towse, J. N., Ellis, D. A., and Towse, A. S. (2021). Opening Pandora’s box: Peeking inside psychology’s data sharing practices, and seven recommendations for change. Behavior Research Methods, 53(4), 14551468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsang, W., Alter, D. A., Wijeysundera, H. C., Zhang, T., and Ko, D. T. (2012). The impact of cardiovascular disease prevalence on women’s enrollment in landmark randomized cardiovascular trials: A systematic review. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 27, 9398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van’t Veer, A. E., and Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Preregistration in social psychology: A discussion and suggested template. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 67, 212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vanpaemel, W., Vermorgen, M., Deriemaecker, L., and Storms, G. (2015). Are we wasting a good crisis? The availability of psychological research data after the storm. Collabra, 1(1), DOI:10.1525/collabra.13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vazire, S., and Holcombe, A. O. (2022). Where are the self-correcting mechanisms in science? Review of General Psychology, 26(2), 212223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagenmakers, E. J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., and van der Maas, H. L. (2011). Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: The case of psi. Comment on Bem (2011). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 426432.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walton, G. M., and Wilson, T. D. (2018). Wise interventions: Psychological remedies for social and personal problems. Psychological Review, 125(5), 617655.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weindling, P. (2001). The origins of informed consent: The International Scientific Commission on Medical War Crimes, and the Nuremberg Code. Bulletin of the History of Medicine, 75, 3771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wendler, D., and Miller, F. G. (2004). Deception in the pursuit of science. Archives of Internal Medicine, 164(6), 597600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wicherts, J. M., Borsboom, D., Kats, J., and Molenaar, D. (2006). The poor availability of psychological research data for reanalysis. American Psychologist, 61(7), 726728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimbardo, P. G., Banks, W. C., Haney, C., and Jaffe, D. (April 8, 1973). The mind is a formidable jailer. New York Times magazine, 38 ff.Google Scholar
Zhou, H., and Fishbach, A. (2016). The pitfall of experimenting on the web: How unattended selective attrition leads to surprising (yet false) research conclusions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111, 493504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Ego depletion and self-control failure: An energy model of the self’s executive function. Self and Identity, 1(2), 129136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., and Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 12521265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., and Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatia, S., and Walasek, L. (2016). Event construal and temporal distance in natural language. Cognition, 152, 18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Becker, D. V., and Srinivasan, N. (2014). The vividness of the happy face. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 189194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertsch, S., Pesta, B. J., Wiscott, R., and McDaniel, M. A. (2007). The generation effect: A meta-analytic review. Memory & Cognition, 35(2), 201210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blázquez, D., Botella, J., and Suero, M. (2017). The debate on the ego-depletion effect: Evidence from meta-analysis with the p-uniform method. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 197, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bullard, S. E., Fein, D., Gleeson, M. K., Tischer, N., Mapou, R. L., and Kaplan, E. (2004). The Biber cognitive estimation test. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6), 835846.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F., Dreber, A., Holzmeister, F., Ho, T.-H., Huber, J., Johannesson, M., Kirchler, M., Nave, G., Nosek, B. A., Pfeiffer, T., Altmejd, A., Buttrick, N., Chan, T., Chen, Y., Forsell, E., Gampa, A., Heikensten, E., Hummer, L., Imai, T., … Wu, H. (2018). Evaluating the replicability of social science experiments in Nature and Science between 2010 and 2015. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(9), 637644, with supplementary material at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0399-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T., and Kenny, D. A. (1999). A Primer on Regression Artifacts. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Vincent, J. E., Lewis, S. K., Catalan, J., Wheeler, D., and Darby, B. L. (1975). Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31(2), 206215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coles, N. A., Larsen, J. T., and Lench, H. C. (2019). A meta-analysis of the facial feedback literature: Effects of facial feedback on emotional experience are small and variable. Psychological Bulletin, 145(6), 610651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coles, N. A., March, D. S., Marmolejo-Ramos, F., Larsen, J., Arinze, N. C., Ndukaihe, I., Willis, M., Francesco, F., Reggev, N., Mokady, A., Forscher, P. S., Hunter, J., Gwenaël, K., Yuvruk, E., Kapucu, A., Nagy, T., Hajdu, N., Tejada, J., Freitag, R., … Marozzi, M. (2022). A multi-lab test of the facial feedback hypothesis by the Many Smiles Collaboration. PsyArXiv, https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/cvpuw.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Combs, B., and Slovic, P. (1979). Newspaper coverage of causes of death. Journalism Quarterly, 56(4), 837849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costello, F., and Watts, P. (2019). The rationality of illusory correlation. Psychological Review, 126(3), 437450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crawford, H. J., Brown, A. M., and Moon, C. E. (1993). Sustained attentional and disattentional abilities: Differences between low and highly hypnotizable persons. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(4), 534543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dang, J. (2018). An updated meta-analysis of the ego depletion effect. Psychological Research, 82(4), 645651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dang, J., Barker, P., Baumert, A., Bentvelzen, M., Berkman, E., Buchholz, N., Buczny, J., Chen, Z., De Cristofaro, V., de Vries, L., Dewitte, S., Giacomantonio, M., Gong, R., Homan, M., Imhoff, R., Ismail, I., Jia, L., Kubiak, T., Lange, F., … Zinkernagel, A. (2021). A multilab replication of the ego depletion effect. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(1), 1424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duhem, P. (1954). The Aim and Structure of Physical Theory (trans. P. Wiener). Atheneum (original work published 1914).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Earp, B. D., and Trafimow, D. (2015). Replication, falsification, and the crisis of confidence in social psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 621, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00621.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feld, G. B., and Born, J. (2015). Exploiting sleep to modify bad attitudes. Science, 348(6238), 971972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance, vol. 2. Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1991). The tricky nature of skewed frequency tables: An information loss account of distinctiveness-based illusory correlations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(1), 2436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., Jung, J., Wänke, M., Alexopoulos, T., and de Molière, L. (2015). Toward a deeper understanding of the ecological origins of distance construal. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 57, 7886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., and Krueger, J. I. (2012). More than an artifact: Regression as a theoretical construct. In Krueger, J. I. (ed.) Social Judgment and Decision Making. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K., Kutzner, F., and Krueger, J. I. (2012). The long way from α-error control to validity proper: Problems with a short-sighted false-positive debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7(6), 661669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., McCaughey, L., and Prager, J. (2021). Quo vadis, methodology? The key role of manipulation checks for validity control and quality of science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 816826.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., and Prager, J. (2018). The regression trap and other pitfalls of replication science: Illustrated by the report of the Open Science Collaboration. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 40(3), 115124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiedler, K., Unkelbach, C., and Freytag, P. (2009). On splitting and merging categories: A regression account of subadditivity. Memory & Cognition, 37(4), 383393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiedler, K., and Walther, E. (2004). Stereotyping as Inductive Hypothesis Testing. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friese, M., Loschelder, D. D., Gieseler, K., Frankenbach, J., and Inzlicht, M. (2019). Is ego depletion real? An analysis of arguments. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(2), 107131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Genschow, O., Westfal, M., Crusius, J., Bartosch, L., Feikes, K. I., Pallasch, N., and Wozniak, M. (2021). Does social psychology persist over half a century? A direct replication of Cialdini et al.’s (1975) classic door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(2), e1e7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L., Alberts, H., Anggono, C. O., Batailler, C., Birt, A. R., … Zwienenberg, M. (2016). A multilab preregistered replication of the ego-depletion effect. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 546573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, D. J., Ellsworth, P. C., and Gonzalez, R. (2018). Are manipulation checks necessary? Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 998, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00998.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hilton, D. J., and Slugoski, B. R. (1986). Knowledge-based causal attribution: The abnormal conditions focus model. Psychological Review, 93, 7588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., and Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 390421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kassin, S. M. (2008). False confessions: Causes, consequences, and implications for reform. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 249253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krüger, T., Fiedler, K., Koch, A. S., and Alves, H. (2014). Response category width as a psychophysical manifestation of construal level and distance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(4), 501512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kruger, J., and Evans, M. (2004). If you don’t want to be late, enumerate: Unpacking reduces the planning fallacy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 586598.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhl, J., and Kazén, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult intentions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive affect removes Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128(3), 382–399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kvarven, A., Strømland, E., and Johannesson, M. (2020). Comparing meta-analyses and preregistered multiple-laboratory replication projects. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(4), 423434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lawrence, D. H., and Festinger, L. (1962). Deterrents and Reinforcement: The Psychology of Insufficient Reward. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (1998). The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(1), 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. F. (1993). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48(5), 518537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McShane, B. B., Tackett, J. L., Böckenholt, U., and Gelman, A. (2019). Large-scale replication projects in contemporary psychological research. American Statistician, 73(sup. 1), 99105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maglio, S. J., and Trope, Y. (2011). Scale and construal: How larger measurement units shrink length estimates and expand mental horizons. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 165170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mongeau, P. A. (1993). The brainstorming myth. Paper presented to the Western States Communication Association, Albuquerque, NM.Google Scholar
Moore, D. A., and Healy, P. J. (2008). The trouble with overconfidence. Psychological Review, 115(2), 502517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morewedge, C. K., Gilbert, D. T., Keysar, B., Berkovits, M. J., and Wilson, T. D. (2007). Mispredicting the hedonic benefits of segregated gains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 700709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., and Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(3), 774789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberauer, K., and Lewandowsky, S. (2019). Addressing the theory crisis in psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(5), 15961618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collaboration, Open Science (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfungst, O. (1911). Clever Hans (The Horse of Mr. Von Osten): A Contribution to Experimental Animal and Human Psychology. Holt, Rinehart andWinston.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polivy, J., and Doyle, C. (1980). Laboratory induction of mood states through the reading of self-referent mood statements: Affective changes or demand characteristics? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89(2), 286290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Proctor, R. W., and Cho, Y. S. (2006). Polarity correspondence: A general principle for performance of speeded binary classification tasks. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 416442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quine, W. V. O. (1952). The Dogmas of Empiricism (reprinted from “A logical point of view”). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rottenstreich, Y., and Tversky, A. (1997). Unpacking, repacking, and anchoring: Advances in support theory. Psychological Review, 104(2), 406415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sagristano, M. D., Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2002). Time-dependent gambling: Odds now, money later. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131(3), 364376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sánchez, A. M., Coleman, C. W., and Ledgerwood, A. (2021). Does temporal distance influence abstraction? A large pre-registered experiment. Social Cognition, 39(3), 352365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1954). The Foundations of Statistics. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. (2014). When and how disgust is and is not implicated in the behavioral immune system. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 8(4), 251256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., and Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 13591366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slamecka, N. J., and Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 592604.Google Scholar
Soderberg, C. K., Callahan, S. P., Kochersberger, A. O., Amit, E., and Ledgerwood, A. (2015). The effects of psychological distance on abstraction: Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 525548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soto, C. J. (2019). How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication project. Psychological Science, 30(5), 711727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, D. J., and Spence, J. R. (2014). Expectations for replications: Are yours realistic? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 305318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storbeck, J., and Clore, G. L. (2008). The affective regulation of cognitive priming. Emotion, 8(2), 208215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., and Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 768777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surowiecki, J. (2004). The Wisdom of Crowds. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
Thorne, K. S. (2019). LIGO and gravitational waves, III: Nobel lecture, December 8, 2017. Annalen der Physik, 531(1), DOI:10.1002/andp.201800350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trafimow, D. (2022). Generalizing across auxiliary, statistical, and inferential assumptions. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 52(1), 3748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A., and Koehler, D. J. (1994). Support theory: A nonextensional representation of subjective probability. Psychological Review, 101, 547567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unkelbach, C. (2006). The learned interpretation of cognitive fluency. Psychological Science, 17(4), 339345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vallacher, R. R., and Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vohs, K. D., Schmeichel, B. J., Lohmann, S., Gronau, Q. F., Finley, A. J., Ainsworth, S. E., Alquist, J. L., Baker, M. D., Brizi, A., Bunyi, A., Butschek, G. J., Campbell, C., Capaldi, J., Cau, C., Chambers, H., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Christensen, W. J., Clay, S. L., Curtis, J., … Albarracín, D. (2021). A multisite preregistered paradigmatic test of the ego-depletion effect. Psychological Science, 32(10), 15661581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Beek, T., Dijkhoff, L., Gronau, Q. F., Acosta, A., Adams, R. B., Albohn, D. N., Allard, E. S., Benning, S. D., Blouin-Hudon, E.-M., Bulnes, L. C., Caldwell, T. L., Calin-Jageman, R. J., Capaldi, C. A., Carfagno, N. S., Chasten, K. T., Cleeremans, A., Connell, L., DeCicco, J. M., … Zwaan, R. A. (2016). Registered replication report: Strack, Martin, & Stepper (1988). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(6), 917928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williamson, D. A., and Blanchard, E. B. (1979). Heart rate and blood pressure biofeedback. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 4(1), 134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Allen, L., O’Connell, A., and Kiermer, V. (2019). How can we ensure visibility and diversity in research contributions? How the Contributor Role Taxonomy (CRediT) is helping the shift from authorship to contributorship. Learned Publishing, 32(1), 7174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheryan, S., Ziegler, S. A., Montoya, A. K., and Jiang, L. (2017). Why are some STEM fields more gender balanced than others? Psychological Bulletin, 143(1), 135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chetty, R., Jackson, M. O., Kuchler, T., Stroebel, J., Hendren, N., Fluegge, R. B., Gong, S., Gonzalez, F., Grondin, A., Jacob, M., Johnston, D., Koenen, M., Laguna-Muggenburg, E., Mudekereza, F., Rutter, T., Thor, N., Townsend, W., Zhang, R., Bailey, M., … Wernerfelt, N. (2022). Social capital I: Measurement and associations with economic mobility. Nature, 608(7921), 108121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christopher, S., Watts, V., McCormick, A. K. H. G., and Young, S. (2008). Building and maintaining trust in a community-based participatory research partnership. American Journal of Public Health, 98(8), 13981406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cikara, M., Martinez, J. E., and Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2022). Moving beyond social categories by incorporating context in social psychological theory. Nature Reviews Psychology.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, S. E., Clifasefi, S. L., Stanton, J., Straits, K. J. E., Espinosa, P. R., Andrasik, M. P., Miller, K. A., Orfaly, V. E., Gil-Kashiwabara, E., Nicasio, A. V., Hawes, S. M., Nelson, L. A., Duran, B. M., and Wallerstein, N. (2018). Community-based participatory research (CBPR): Towards equitable involvement of community in psychology research. American Psychologist, 73(7), 884898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, S. J., Young, N., Donaldson, M. R., Nyboer, E. A., Roche, D. G., Madliger, C. L., Lennox, R. J., Chapman, J. M., Faulkes, Z., and Bennett, J. R. (2021). Ten strategies for avoiding and overcoming authorship conflicts in academic publishing. FACETS, 6, 17531770.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dupree, C. H., and Kraus, M. W. (2022). Psychological science is not race neutral. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(1), 270275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eaton, A. A., Grzanka, P. R., Schlehofer, M. M., and Silka, L. (2021). Public psychology: Introduction to the special issue. American Psychologist, 76(8), 12091216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eggert, L. (2011). Best practices for allocating appropriate credit and responsibility to authors of multi-authored articles. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forscher, P. S., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Coles, N. A., Silan, M. A., Dutra, N., Basnight-Brown, D. M., and IJzerman, H. (in press). The benefits, barriers, and risks of big team science. Perspectives on Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., and Schuh, E. S. (1994). An ethnic bias in scientific citations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(6), 623639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gruber, J., Mendle, J., Lindquist, K. A., Schmader, T., Clark, L. A., Bliss-Moreau, E., Akinola, M., Atlas, L., Barch, D. M., Barrett, L. F., Borelli, J. L., Brannon, T. N., Bunge, S. A., Campos, B., Cantlon, J., Carter, R., Carter-Sowell, A. R., Chen, S., Craske, M. G., … Williams, L. A. (2021). The future of women in psychological science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(3), 483516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grzanka, P. R., and Cole, E. R. (2021). An argument for bad psychology: Disciplinary disruption, public engagement, and social transformation. American Psychologist, 76(8), 13341345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, K., Vogel, A., Huang, G., Serrano, K., Rice, E., Tsakraklides, S., and Fiore, S. (2018). The science of team science: A review of the empirical evidence and research gaps on collaboration in science. American Psychologist, 73, 532548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., and Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IJzerman, H., Lewis, N. A., Jr., Przybylski, A. K., Weinstein, N., DeBruine, L., Ritchie, S. J., Vazire, S., Forscher, P. S., Morey, R. D., Ivory, J. D., and Anvari, F. (2020). Use caution when applying behavioural science to policy. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(11), 10921094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jachimowicz, J. M. (2022). Embracing field studies as a tool for learning. Nature Reviews Psychology, 1(5), 249250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Flake, J. K., Liuzza, M. T., Antfolk, J., Arinze, N. C., Ndukaihe, I. L. G., Bloxsom, N. G., Lewis, S. C., Foroni, F., Willis, M. L., Cubillas, C. P., Vadillo, M. A., Turiegano, E., Gilead, M., Simchon, A., Saribay, S. A., Owsley, N. C., Jang, C., … Coles, N. A. (2021). To which world regions does the valence–dominance model of social perception apply? Nature Human Behaviour, 5(1), 159169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, G. (2007). Performing a project premortem. Harvard Business Review, 85, 1819.Google Scholar
Klein, R. A., Cook, C. L., Ebersole, C. R., Vitiello, C., Nosek, B. A., Hilgard, J., Ahn, P. H., Brady, A. J., Chartier, C. R., Christopherson, C. D., Clay, S., Collisson, B., Crawford, J. T., Cromar, R., Gardiner, G., Gosnell, C. L., Grahe, J., Hall, C., Howard, I., … Ratliff, K. A. (2022). Many labs 4: Failure to replicate mortality salience effect with and without original author involvement. Collabra: Psychology, 8(1), 35271, https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.35271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, R. A., Vianello, M., Hasselman, F., Adams, B. G., Adams, R. B., Alper, S., Aveyard, M., Axt, J. R., Babalola, M. T., Bahník, Š. , Batra, R., Berkics, M., Bernstein, M. J., Berry, D. R., Bialobrzeska, O., Binan, E. D., Bocian, K., Brandt, M. J., Busching, R., … Nosek, B. A. (2018). Many labs 2: Investigating variation in replicability across samples and settings. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 443490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., Carroll, T. J., Karapetyan, A., Kaushik, N., Tomezsko, D., Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, M. R. (2019). Relationship between the implicit association test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larrick, R. P., Mannes, A. E., and Soll, J. B. (2012). The social psychology of the wisdom of crowds. In Joachim I. Krueger (ed.) Social Judgment and Decision Making. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ledgerwood, A., Hudson, S. T. J., Lewis, N. A., Jr., Maddox, K. B., Pickett, C. L., Remedios, J. D., Cheryan, S., Diekman, A. B., Dutra, N. B., Goh, J. X., Goodwin, S. A., Munakata, Y., Navarro, D. J., Onyeador, I. N., Srivastava, S., and Wilkins, C. L. (2022). The pandemic as a portal: Reimagining psychological science as truly open and inclusive. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17456916211036654, https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211036654.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledgerwood, A., Pickett, C., Navarro, D., Remedios, J. D., and Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2022). The unbearable limitations of solo science: Team science as a path for more rigorous and relevant research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 45, e81, https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X21000844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2021a). Can we achieve “equality” when we have different understandings of its meaning? How contexts and identities shape the pursuit of egalitarian goals. Psychological Inquiry, 32(3), 155164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2021b). What counts as good science? How the battle for methodological legitimacy affects public psychology. American Psychologist, 76(8), 13231333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2022). What universities say versus do about diversity, equity and inclusion. Nature Human Behaviour, 6(5), 610–610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lin, Z., and Li, N. (2022). Global diversity of authors, editors, and journal ownership across subdisciplines of psychology: Current state and policy implications. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17456916221091832, https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916221091831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., and Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27(1), 415444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McShane, B. B., Tackett, J. L., Böckenholt, U., and Gelman, A. (2019). Large-scale replication projects in contemporary psychological research. American Statistician, 73(sup1), 99105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, M. N., Heck, P. R., Holtzman, G. S., Anderson, S. M., Cai, W., Watts, D. J., and Chabris, C. F. (2019). Objecting to experiments that compare two unobjectionable policies or treatments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(22), 1072310728.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milkman, K. L., Gromet, D., Ho, H., Kay, J. S., Lee, T. W., Pandiloski, P., Park, Y., Rai, A., Bazerman, M., Beshears, J., Bonacorsi, L., Camerer, C., Chang, E., Chapman, G., Cialdini, R., Dai, H., Eskreis-Winkler, L., Fishbach, A., Gross, J. J., … Duckworth, A. L. (2021). Megastudies improve the impact of applied behavioural science. Nature, 600(7889), 478483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, J., Boettcher-Sheard, N., Duque, C., and Lashewicz, B. (2018). Who do we think we are? Disrupting notions of quality in qualitative research. Qualitative Health Research, 28(4), 673680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moshontz, H., Campbell, L., Ebersole, C. R., IJzerman, H., Urry, H. L., Forscher, P. S., Grahe, J. E., McCarthy, R. J., Musser, E. D., Antfolk, J., Castille, C. M., Evans, T. R., Fiedler, S., Flake, J. K., Forero, D. A., Janssen, S. M. J., Keene, J. R., Protzko, J., Aczel, B., … Chartier, C. R. (2018). The Psychological Science Accelerator: Advancing psychology through a distributed collaborative network. Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 1(4), 501515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, M. E. J. (2001). The structure of scientific collaboration networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 404409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D. (2015). Culture as situated cognition. In Robert A. Scott and Marlis C. Buchmann (gen. eds) and Stephen M. Kosslyn (consulting ed.) Emerging Trends in the Social and Behavioral Sciences: An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0067.Google Scholar
Page, S. E. (2019). The Diversity Bonus: How Great Teams Pay Off in the Knowledge Economy. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Paluck, E. L., and Cialdini, R. B. (2013). Field research methods. In Reis, H. T. and Judd, C. M. (eds) Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Pearson, A. R., White, K. E., Nogueira, L., Lewis, N. A., Jr., Green, D. J., Schuldt, J. P., and Edmondson, D. (in press). Climate change and health equity: A research agenda for psychological science. American Psychologist.Google Scholar
Posselt, J. R. (2016). Inside Graduate Admissions: Merit, Diversity, and Faculty Gatekeeping. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reece, A., Cooney, G., Bull, P., Chung, C., Dawson, B., Fitzpatrick, C., Glazer, T., Knox, D., Liebscher, A., and Marin, S. (2022). Advancing an interdisciplinary science of conversation: Insights from a large multimodal corpus of human speech (arXiv:2203.00674). arXiv, https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2203.00674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, S. O., Bareket-Shavit, C., Dollins, F. A., Goldie, P. D., and Mortenson, E. (2020). Racial inequality in psychological research: Trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(6), 12951309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Settles, I. H., Jones, M. K., Buchanan, N. T., and Dotson, K. (2021). Epistemic exclusion: Scholar(ly) devaluation that marginalizes faculty of color. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 14(4), 493507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starck, J. G., Sinclair, S., and Shelton, J. N. (2021). How university diversity rationales inform student preferences and outcomes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(16), e2013833118, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2013833118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syed, M. (2021). It’s 2 × 2 designs all the way down: Social psychology’s over-reliance on experiments restricts diversity in the field (conference presentation), https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/u89e2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., Crockett, M. J., Crum, A. J., Douglas, K. M., Druckman, J. N., Drury, J., Dube, O., Ellemers, N., Finkel, E. J., Fowler, J. H., Gelfand, M., Han, S., Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yudkin, D. A., Stolier, R. M., and Lewis, N. A., Jr. (2021). Changing perceptions around nontraditional career trajectories in psychological science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 25(6), 417418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuberi, T., and Bonilla-Silva, E. (eds.). (2008). White Logic, White Methods: Racism and Methodology. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar

References

Adams, R. B., Jr., Rule, N. O., Franklin, R. G. Jr., Wang, E., Stevenson, M. T., Yoshikawa, S., Nomura, M., Sato, W., Kveraga, K., and Ambady, N. (2009). Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 97108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alter, A. L., and Kwan, V. S. Y. (2009). Cultural sharing in a global village: Evidence for extracultural cognition in European Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(4), 742760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Althoff, T., Sosič, R., Hicks, J. King, L., Delp, A. C., S., and Leskovec, J. (2017). Large-scale physical activity data reveal worldwide activity inequality. Nature, 547, 336339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., and Akert, R. M. (2010). Social psychology, 7th ed. Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Ashman, O., Shiomura, K., and Levy, B. R. (2006). Influence of culture and age on control beliefs: The missing link of interdependence. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 62(2), 143157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ateca-Amestoy, V., Aguilar, A. C., and Moro-Egido, A. (2014). Social interactions and life satisfaction: Evidence from Latin America. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15, 527554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bagozzi, R. P., Wong, N., and Yi, Y. (1999). The role of culture and gender in the relationship between positive and negative affect. Cognition and Emotion, 13(6), 641672.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benet-Martínez, V., Leu, J., Lee, F., and Morris, M. W. (2002). Negotiating biculturalism: Cultural frame switching in biculturals with oppositional versus compatible cultural identities. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 33, 492516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biswas-Diener, R., and Diener, E. (2001). Making the best of a bad situation: Satisfaction in the slums of Calcutta. Social Indicators Research, 55, 329352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B., and Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “We”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 8393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brislin, R. W. (1986).The wording and translation of research instruments. In Lonner, W. J. and Berry, J. W. (eds) Field Methods in Cross-cultural Research, vol. 8, Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Brislin, R. W., Lonner, W. J., and Thorndike, R. M. (1973). Cross-cultural Research Methods. Wiley.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., Mischel, W., and Schwartz, J. (1982). A prototype analysis of psychological situations. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 4577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, T. E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2022). Word embeddings reveal social group attitudes and stereotypes in large language corpora. In Dehghani, M. & Boyd, R. (eds.), Handbook of language analysis in psychology (pp. 494510). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., and Dzokoto, V. (2014). Listen to your heart: The cultural shaping of interoceptive awareness and accuracy. Emotion, 14, 666678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheryan, S., and Monin, B. (2005). Where are you really from? Asian Americans and identity denial. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(5), 717730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, C., Gelfand, M. J., Yamagishi, T., Shteynberg, G., and Wan, C. (2010). Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective perceptions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 482493.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, J., Catapano, R., and Choi, I. (2017). Taking stock of happiness and meaning in everyday life: An experience sampling approach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(6), 641651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, I., Dalal, R., Kim-Prieto, C., and Park, H. (2003). Culture and judgment of causal relevance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 4659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, D. (1996). Law, social policy, and violence: The impact of regional cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(5), 961978.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D. (2003). The American national conversation about (everything but) shame. Social Research: An International Quarterly, 70 (4), 10751108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., and Nisbett, R. E. (1997). Field experiments examining the culture of honor: The role of institutions in perpetuating norms about violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 11881199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., Nisbett, R. E., Bowdle, B. F., and Schwarz, N. (1996). Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An experimental ethnography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 945960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1995). Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality assessment using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Personality Assessment, 64(1), 2150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, S., Uskul, A. K., Gercek-Swing, B., Sunbay, Z., Ataca, B., and Karakitapoglu, Z. (2014). Cultural prototypes and dimensions of honor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 232249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., and Diener, M. (1995). Cross-cultural correlates of life satisfaction and self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(4), 653663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., and Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 7175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagly, A. H., and Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54(6), 408423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebert, T., Götz, F. M., Gladstone, J. J., Müller, S. R., and Matz, S. C. (2021). Spending reflects not only who we are but also who we are around: The joint effects of individual and geographic personality on consumption. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(2), 378393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., and Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festinger, L., and Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive consequences of forced compliance. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58(2), 203210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, R. (2004). Standardization to account for cross-cultural response bias: A classification of score adjustment procedures and review of research in JCCP. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 35(3), 263282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, R. (2006). Congruence and functions of personal and cultural values: Do my values reflect my culture’s values? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(1), 14191431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funder, D. C. (2019). The Personality Puzzle, 8th ed. W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Gawronski, B. (2004). Theory-based bias correction in dispositional inference: The fundamental attribution error is dead, long live the correspondence bias. European Review of Social Psychology, 15,183217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C. et al. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 11001104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gladstone, J. J., Matz, S. C., and Lemaire, A. (2019). Can psychological traits be inferred from spending? Evidence from transaction data. Psychological Science, 30, 10871096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golder, S. A., and Macy, M. W. (2011). Diurnal and seasonal mood vary with work, sleep, and daylength across diverse cultures. Science, 333, 18781881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The changing psychology of culture from 1800 through 2000. Psychological Science, 24(9), 17221731.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., and Childs, C. P. (2003). Historical change, cultural learning, and cognitive representation in Zinacantec Maya children. Cognitive Development, 18, 455487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Günsoy, C., Joo, M., Cross, S. E., Uskul, A. K., Gul, P., Wasti, S. A., Salter, P., Haugen, A., Erdas, D., and Yegin, A. (2020). The influence of honor threats on goal delay and goal derailment: A comparison of Turkey, southern U.S., and northern U.S. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 88, 103974.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurven, M., von Rueden, C., Massenkoff, M., Kaplan, H., and Lero Vie, M. (2013). How universal is the Big Five? Testing the five-factor model of personality variation among forager–farmers in the Bolivian Amazon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104(2), 354370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, D. W., Lee, K., and Ashton, M. C. (1999). A factor analysis of the most frequently used Korean personality trait adjectives. European Journal of Personality, 13, 261282.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16(1), 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, R. S., and Varnum, M. E. W. (2018). Do cultures vary in self-enhancement? ERP, behavioral, and self-report evidence. Social Neuroscience, 13(5), 566578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, S.-P., and Shavitt, S. (1994). Persuasion and culture: Advertising appeals in individualistic and collectivistic societies. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30(4), 326350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkness, J. A, Pennell, B-E. , and Schoua-Glusberg, A. (2004). Survey questionnaire translation and assessment. In Presser, S., Rothgeb, J., Couper, M., Lessler, J., Martin, E., Martin, J., and Singer, E. (eds) Methods for Testing and Evaluating Survey Questionnaires. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
He, J., and van de Vijver, F. (2012). Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural research. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(2), https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., and Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(4), 599615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heine, S. J., and Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 389400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Peng, K., and Greenholtz, J. (2002). What’s wrong with cross-cultural comparisons of subjective Likert scales? The reference-group effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 903918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, B., Thoni, C., and Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 13621367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hong, Y.-Y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-Y., and Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55, 709720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoshino-Browne, E., Zanna, A. S., Spencer, S. J., Zanna, M. P., Kitayama, S., and Lackenbauer, S. (2005). On the cultural guises of cognitive dissonance: The case of Easterners and Westerners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 294310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackson, J. C., Watts, J., Henry, T. R., List, J.-M., Forkel, R., Muncha, P. J., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., and Lindquist, K. A. (2019). Emotion semantics show both cultural variation and universal structure. Science, 366, 15171522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Flake, J. K., Liuzza, M. T.Coles, N. A. (2021). To which world regions does the valence–dominance model of social perception apply? Nature Human Behavior, 5, 159169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kashima, E. S., and Kashima, Y. (1998). Culture and language: The case of cultural dimensions and personal pronoun use. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 29(3), 461486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y., and Triandis, H. C. (1986). The self-serving bias in attributions as a coping strategy: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 17(1), 8397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., Napier, J. L., Callan, M. J., and Laurin, K. (2008). God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(1), 1835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, H., Borke, J., Chaudhary, N., Lamm, B., and Kleis, A. (2010). Continuity in parenting strategies: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 41(3), 391409.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, H., Kärtner, J., Borke, J., Yovsi, R., and Kleis, A. (2005). Parenting styles and the development of the categorical self: A longitudinal study on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 496504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemmelmeier, M., and Cheng, B. Y.-M. (2004). Language and self-construal priming: A replication and extension in a Hong Kong sample. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 35(6), 705712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., and Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition & Emotion, 14(1), 93124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., and Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 12451267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraemer, M. U. G., Sadilek, A., Zhang, Q., and Brownstein, J. S. (2020). Mapping global variation in human mobility. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 800810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraus, M. W., Callaghan, B., and Ondish, P. (2019). Social class as culture. In Cohen, D. and Kitayama, S. (eds) Handbook of Cultural Psychology. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kubota, S., Onishi, K., and Toyama, Y. (2021). Consumption responses to COVID-19 payments: Evidence from a natural experiment and bank account data. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 188, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krys, K., Vignoles, V., Uchida, Y., and de Almeida, I. (2022). Outside the “cultural binary”: Understanding why Latin American collectivist societies foster independent selves. Perspectives on Psychological Science. Advance online publication, https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211029632.Google Scholar
Lam, A. G., and Zane, N. W. S. (2004). Ethnic differences in coping with interpersonal stressors: A test of self-construals as cultural mediators. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 35(4), 446459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, K., and Ashton, M. C. (2018). Psychometric properties of the HEXACO-100. Assessment, 25, 543556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leung, K., and Morris, M. W. (2015). Values, schemas and norms in the culture–behavior nexus: A situated dynamics framework. Journal of International Business Studies, 46(9), 10281050.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, R. V., and Norenzayan, A. (1999). The pace of life in 31 countries. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 30(2), 178205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, R. V., Norenzayan, A., and Philbrick, K. (2001). Cross-cultural differences in helping strangers. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 32(5), 543560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Y. J., Johnson, K. A., Cohen, A. B., Williams, M. J., Knowles, E. D., and Chen, Z. (2012). Fundamental(ist) attribution error: Protestants are dispositionally focused. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(2), 281290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Little, T. D. (1997). Mean and covariance structures (MACS) analyses of cross-cultural data: Practical and theoretical issues. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 32, 5376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lomas, T. (2016). Towards a positive cross-cultural lexicography: Enriching our emotional landscape through 216 “untranslatable” words pertaining to well-being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 11, 546558.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., and Kaushanskaya, M. (2004). Self-construal and emotion in bicultural bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(2), 190201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R., Kitayama, S., and Heiman, R. J. (1996). Culture and basic psychological principles. In Higgins, E. T. and Kruglanski, A. W. (eds) Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. Guilford.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., Uchida, Y., Omoregie, H., Townsend, S. S. M., and Kitayama, S. (2006). Going for the gold: Models of agency in Japanese and American contexts. Psychological Science, 17(2), 103112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masuda, T., Gonzalez, R., Kwan, L., and Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Culture and aesthetic preference: Comparing the attention to context of East Asians and Americans. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 12601275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesquita, B., and Karasawa, M. (2002). Different emotional lives. Cognition & Emotion, 16(1), 127141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., and Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3), 311323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morling, B., and Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism–collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 199221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, M. W., and Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949971.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., and Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norenzayan, A., and Heine, S. J. (2005). Psychological universals: What are they and how can we know? Psychological Bulletin, 131(5), 763784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oishi, S. (2006). The concept of life satisfaction across cultures: An IRT analysis. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 411423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Choi, D. W., Kim-Prieto, C., and Choi, I. (2007). The dynamics of daily events and well-being across cultures: When less is more. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 685698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Scollon, C. N., and Biswas-Diener, R. (2004). Cross-situational consistency of affective experiences across cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 460472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oishi, S., Graham, J., Kesebir, S., and Galinha, I. (2013). Concepts of happiness across time and cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 559577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogihara, Y., Fujita, H., Tominaga, H., Ishigaki, S., Kashimoto, T., Takahashi, A., Toyohara, K., and Uchida, Y. (2015). Are common names becoming less common? The rise in uniqueness and individualism in Japan. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1490, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oosternhof, N. N., and Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(32), 1108711092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., and Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, B., Qu, Y., Chim, L., Blevins, E., Knutson, B., and Tsai, J. L. (2018). Ventral striatal activity mediates cultural differences in affiliative judgments of smiles. Culture and Brain, 6, 102117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeves, B., Ram, N., Robinson, T. N., Cummings, J. J., Giles, C. L. et al. (2019). Screenomics: A framework to capture and analyze personal life experiences and the ways that technology shapes them. Human–Computer Interaction, 36(2), 150201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L., and Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The Person and the Situation: Perspectives of Social Psychology . Mcgraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Ross, L. D. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Berkowitz, L. (ed.) Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, vol. 10. Random House.Google Scholar
Sabini, J., Siepmann, M., and Stein, J. (2001). The really fundamental attribution error in social psychological research. Psychological Inquiry, 12(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salvador, C. E., Kamikubo, A., Kraus, B., Hsiao, N.-C., Hu, J.-F., Karasawa, M., and Kitayama, S. (2021). Self-referential processing accounts for cultural variation in self-enhancement versus criticism: An electrocortical investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. Advance online publication, https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0001154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
San Martin, A., Sinaceur, M., Madi, A., Tompson, S., Maddux, W. W., and Kitayama, S. (2018). Self-assertive interdependence in Arab culture. Nature Human Behavior, 2(11), 830837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, G., and Goldberg, L. R. (2001). Lexical studies of indigenous personality factors: Premises, products, and prospects. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 847879.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U., Oishi, S., and Diener, E. (2002). Cultural influences on the relation between pleasant emotions and unpleasant emotions: Asian dialectic philosophies or individualism–collectivism? Cognition and Emotion, 16, 705719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schimmack, U., Radhakrishnan, P., Oishi, S., Dzokoto, V., and Ahadi, S. (2002). Culture, personality, and subjective well-being: Integrating process models of life satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 582593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R., and Phillips, L. T. (2014). Social class culture cycles: How three gateway contexts shape selves and fuel inequality. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 611634.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sugiyama, L. S. (2004). Is beauty in the context-sensitive adaptations of the beholder? Shiwiar use of waist-to-hip ratio in assessments of female mate value. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(1), 5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suh, E., Diener, E., Oishi, S., and Triandis, H. C. (1998). The shifting basis of life satisfaction judgments across cultures: Emotions versus norms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(2), 482493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, L., Axhausen, K. W., Lee, D.-H., and Huang, X. (2013). Understanding metropolitan patterns of daily encounters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(34), 1377413779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szarota, P. (1996). Taxonomy of the Polish personality-descriptive adjectives of the highest frequency of use. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 27, 342351.Google Scholar
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., and Oishi, S. (2018). Moving chairs in Starbucks: Observational studies find rice–wheat cultural differences in daily life in China. Science Advances, 4(4), eaap8469. DOI:10.1126/sciadv.aap8469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., and Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344, 603608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, S. E., and Fiske, S. T. (1975). Point of view and perceptions of causality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32(3), 439445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, R., Yuki, M., Talhelm, T., … Visserman, M. L. (2018). Relational mobility predicts social behaviors in 39 countries and is tied to historical farming and threat. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(29), 75217526.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J. L., and Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(33), 1165511660.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J. L., Shariff, A. F., Zhao, W., and Henrich, J. (2013). Cross-cultural evidence that the nonverbal expression of pride is an automatic status signal. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 163180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., and Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(5), 649655.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Triandis, H. C., Marín, G., Lisansky, J., and Betancourt, H. (1984). Simpatía as a cultural script of Hispanics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 13631375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsai, J. L., Louie, J. Y., Chen, E. E., and Uchida, Y. (2007). Learning what feelings to desire: Socialization of ideal affect through children’s storybooks. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(1), 1730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsai, J. L., Miao, F. F., and Seppala, E. (2007). Good feelings in Christianity and Buddhism: Religious differences in ideal affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(3), 409421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M., Abebe, E. M., and Campbell, W. K. (2010). Fitting in or standing out: trends in American parents’ choices for children’s names, 1880–2007. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 1, 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uchida, Y., and Kitayama, S. (2009). Happiness and unhappiness in East and West: Themes and variations. Emotion, 9(4), 441456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uskul, A. K., Cross, S., Gercek-Swing, B., Sunbay, Z., and Ataca, B. (2012). Honor bound: The cultural construction of honor in Turkey and the northern US. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 43, 11311151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uskul, A. K., Hynie, M., and Lalonde, R. N. (2004). Interdependence as a mediator between culture and interpersonal closeness for Euro-Canadians and Turks. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 35(2), 174191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uskul, A. K., Kitayama, S., and Nisbett, R. E. (2008). Ecocultural basis of cognition: Farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(25), 85528556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uskul, A. K., Oyserman, D., Schwarz, N., Lee, S. W. S., and Xu, A. J. (2013). How successful you have been in life depends on the response scale used: The role of cultural mindsets in pragmatic inferences drawn from question format. Social Cognition, 31(2), 222236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van de Vijver, F. (1998). Towards a theory of bias and equivalence. In Harkness, J. A. (ed.) ZUMA-Nachrichten Spezial, vol. 3, Cross-cultural Survey Equivalence. Zuma.Google Scholar
van de Vijver, F., and Tanzer, N. K. (2004). Bias and equivalence in cross-cultural assessment: An overview. European Review of Applied Psychology/Revue européenne de psychologie appliquée, 54(2), 119135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W., and Kitayama, S. (2011). What’s in a name? Popular names are less common on frontiers. Psychological Science, 22(2), 176183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vazire, S., Schiavone, S. R., and Bottesini, J. G. (2022). Credibility beyond replicability: Improving the four validities in psychological science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(2), 162168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vignoles, V., Owe, E., Becker, M., Smith, P., Easterbrook, M. J., Brown, R., González, R., Didier, N., Carrasco, D., Cadena, M. P., Lay, S., Schwartz, S. J., Des Rosiers, S. E., Villamar, J. A., Gavreliuc, A., Zinkeng, M., Kreuzbauer, R., Baguma, P., Martin, M., … Bond, M. H. (2016). Beyond the “East–West” dichotomy: Global variation in cultural models of selfhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 9661000.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, L., Reis, H. T., and Bond, M. H. (1989). Collectivism–individualism in everyday social life: The middle kingdom and the melting pot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 7986.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, Wright, C. (1959). The sociological imagination. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Yu, F., Peng, T., Peng, K., Tang, S., Chen, C. S., Qian, X., Sun, P., Han, T., and Chai, F. (2016). Cultural value shifting in pronoun use. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 47(2), 310316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Y., Lai, M. H. C., and Palardy, G. J. (2022). A Bayesian region of measurement equivalence (ROME) approach for establishing measurement invariance. Psychological Methods. Advance online publication, https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000455.Google Scholar
Zou, X., Tam, K., Morris, W. M., Lee, L., Lau, I., and Chiu, C. Y. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus vs. personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 579597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Attribution of success and failure revisited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attribution theory. Journal of Personality, 47(2), 245287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×