Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T01:06:32.745Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Early color symbolism

from Part V - Color symbolism and association

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2016

Andrew J. Elliot
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Mark D. Fairchild
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Anna Franklin
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aiello, L., and Dunbar, R. (1993). Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Current Anthropology, 34, 184–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alcorta, C., and Sosis, R. (2005). Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: the evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Human Nature, 16, 323–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barham, L. (2002). Systematic pigment use in the Middle Pleistocene of south central Africa. Current Anthropology, 43, 181–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beall, A., and Tracy, J. (2013). Woman are more likely to wear red or pink at peak fertility. Psychological Science, 24, 1837–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beaumont, P. (1973). The ancient pigment mines of southern Africa. South African Journal of Science, 69, 41–6.Google Scholar
Beaumont, P., and Bednarik, R. (2013). Tracing the emergence of palaeoart in sub-Saharan Africa. Rock Art Research, 30, 3354.Google Scholar
Bell, D. (1983). Daughters of the Dreaming. Melbourne: McPhee Gribble/Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Berlin, B., and Kay, P. (1969). Basic Color Terms: Their Universality and Evolution. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Berna, F., Goldberg, P., Horwitz, L., Brink, J., Hold, S., Bamford, M., and Chazan, M. (2012). Microstratigraphic evidence of in situ fire in the Acheulean strata of Wonderwerk Cave, Northern Cape Province, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, E1215–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Bird-Davis, N. (2006). Animistic epistemology: why do some hunter-gatherers not depict animals? Ethnos, 71, 3350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bordes, F. (1952). Sur l’usage probable de la peinture corporelle dans certaines tribus moustériennes. Bulletin Societé Préhistoire Française, 49, 169–71.Google Scholar
Bowles, S. (2006). Group competition, reproductive levelling, and the evolution of altruism. Science, 314, 1569–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S., and Dissanayake, E. (2009). The arts are more than aesthetics: neuroaesthetics as narrow aesthetics. In Skov, M. and Vartanian, O. (eds.), Neuroaesthetics (pp. 4357). Amityville, NY: Baywood.Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J. (2007). The evolution of religion. In Dunbar, R. (ed.), Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (pp. 621–35). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J., and Sosis, R. (2011). Signalling theory and the evolution of religious cooperation. Religion, 41, 363–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burdukiewicz, J. (2014). The origin of symbolic behavior of Middle Palaeolithic humans: recent controversies. Quaternary International, 326–7, 398405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J., Hrdy, S., and van Schaik, C. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 175–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, P. (1994). On symbols and the Palaeolithic. Current Anthropology, 35, 627–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, P., and Dibble, H. (1987). Middle Paleolithic symbolism: a review of current evidence and interpretations. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 6, 263–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chazan, M. (2009). Assessing the Lower to Middle Paleolithic transition. In Camps, M. and Chauhan, P. (eds.), Sourcebook of Paleolithic Transitions (pp. 237–43). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Chazan, M., and Horwitz, L. (2009). Milestones in the development of symbolic behaviour: a case study from Wonderwerk Cave, South Africa. World Archaeology, 41, 521–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chazan, M., Porat, N., Sumner, T., and Horwitz, L. (2013). The use of OSL dating in unstructured sands: the archaeology and chronology of the Hutton Sands at Canteen Kopje (Northern Cape Province, South Africa). Archaeological and Anthropological Sciences, 5, 351–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, J. D. (1988). The Middle Stone Age of East Africa and the beginnings of regional identity. Journal of World Prehistory, 2, 237305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coltorti, M., Feraud, G., Marzoli, A., Peretto, C., Ton-That, T., Voinchet, P., Bahain, J.-J., et al. (2005). New 40Ar/39Ar, stratigraphic and palaeoclimatic data on the Isernia La Pineta Lower Palaeolithic site, Molise, Italy. Quarternary International, 131, 1122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conard, N. (2005). An overview of patterns of behavioral change in Africa and Eurasia during the Middle and Late Pleistocence. In d’Errico, F. and Blackwell, L. (eds.), From Early Hominids to Modern Humans (pp. 294332). Johannesburg: Wits University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, R., Smolker, R., and Bejder, L. (2006). Synchrony, social behaviour and alliance affiliation in Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops aduncus. Animal Behaviour, 72, 1371–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cornell, R.M., and Schwertmann, U. (2003). The Iron Oxides: Structure, Properties, Reactions, Occurrence and Uses. Weinheim: Wiley-VCH,CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S., Staurset, S., and Walker, N. (2011). Ritualised behavior in the Middle Stone Age: evidence from Rhino Cave, Tsodillo Hills, Botswana. PaleoAnthropology, 2011, 1861.Google Scholar
Cremaschi, M., and Peretto, C. (1988). Les sols d’habitat du site paléolithique d’Isernia la Pineta (Molise, Italie centrale). L’Anthropologie, 92, 1017–40.Google Scholar
Deacon, H. (1995). Two late Pleistocene-Holocene archaeological depositories from the Southern Cape, South Africa. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 50, 121–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Human Brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Dediu, D., and Levinson, S. (2013). On the antiquity of language: the reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dediu, D., and Levinson, S. (2014). The time frame of the emergence of modern language and its implications. In Dor, D., Lewis, J., and Knight, C. (eds.), The Social Evolution of Language (pp. 184–95). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Delhey, K., Peters, A., and Kempanaers, B. (2007). Cosmetic coloration in birds: occurrence, function, and evolution. American Naturalist, 169, S145–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Lumley, H. (1966). Les fouilles de Terra Amata à Nice. Bulletin du Musée d’Anthropologie Préhistorique de Monaco, 13, 2951.Google Scholar
de Miguel, C., and Hennerberg, M. (2001). Variation in hominid brain size: how much is due to method? Homo, 52, 358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
d’Errico, F. (2003). The invisible frontier: a multiple species model for the origin of behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 188202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d’Errico, F., and Banks, W. (2013). Identifying mechanisms behind the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age cultural trajectories. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S371–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d’Errico, F., and Henshilwood, C. (2011). The origin of symbolically mediated behaviour: from antagonistic scenarios to a unified research strategy. In Henshilwood, C. and d’Errico, F. (eds.), Homo symbolicus: The Dawn of Language, Imagination and Spirituality (pp. 4974). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
d’Errico, F., and Stringer, C. (2011). Evolution, revolution or saltation scenario for the emergence of modern culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 1060–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo aestheticus: Where Art Comes from and Why. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2009). The artifaction hypothesis and its relevance to cognitive science, evolutionary aesthetics, and neuroaesthetics. Cognitive Semiotics, 5, 148–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubreuil, B., and Henshilwood, C. (2013). Archaeology and the language-ready brain. Language and Cognition, 5, 251–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1999). Culture, honesty and the freerider problem. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C., and Power, C. (eds.), The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View (pp. 147–69). Edinburgh University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, R. (2003). The social brain: mind, language, and society in evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32, 163–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1961/1915). The Elementary Forms of Religious Life: A Study in Religious Sociology, trans. Swain, J. W.. New York: Collier Books.Google Scholar
Elliot, A., and Maier, M. (2012). Color-in-context theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 63125.Google Scholar
Elliot, A., and Niesta, D. (2008). Romantic red: red enhances men’s attraction to women. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1150–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feblot-Augustins, J. (1990). Exploitation des matières premières dans l’Acheuléen d’Afrique: perspectives comportementales. Paléo, 2, 2742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finnegan, M. (2013). The politics of Eros: ritual dialogue and egalitarianism in three central African hunter-gatherer societies. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 19, 697715.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henshilwood, C., d’Errico, F., and Watts, I. (2009). Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 57(1), 2747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodgskiss, T. (2012). An investigation into the properties of the ochre from Sibudu, KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa. Southern African Humanities, 24, 99120.Google Scholar
Hodgson, D. (2011). The first appearance of symmetry in the human lineage: where perception meets art. Symmetry, 3, 3753.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hovers, E., Ilani, S., Bar-Yosef, O., and Vandermeersch, B. (2003). An early case of color symbolism: ochre use by modern humans in Qafzeh Cave. Current Anthropology, 44, 491522.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. (2009). Mothers and Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jehs, D. (2011). The modernity myth. Notae Praehistoricae, 31, 6985.Google Scholar
Kay, P., and Maffi, L. (1999). Color appearance and the emergence and evolution of basic color lexicons. American Anthropologist, 101, 743–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., and McDaniel, C. (1978). The linguistic significance of the meanings of basic color terms. Language, 54, 610–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knight, C. (1991). Blood Relations: Menstruation and the Origins of Culture. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Knight, C. (1998). Ritual/speech coevolution: a solution to the problem of deception. In Hurfurd, J., Studdert-Kennedy, M., and Knight, C. (eds.), Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases (pp. 6891). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Knight, C. (2014). Language and symbolic culture: an outcome of hunter-gatherer reverse dominance. In Dor, D., Lewis, J., and Knight, C. (eds.), The Social Evolution of Language (pp. 228–47). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Knight, C, Power, C., and Watts, I. (1995). The human symbolic revolution: a Darwinian account. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 5, 75114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knight, C, Power, C., and Watts, I. (2003). Comment on Hovers et al. 2003. Current Anthropology, 44, 513–14.Google Scholar
Kuhn, S. (2014). Signalling theory and technologies of communication in the Paleolithic. Biological Theory, 9, 4250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, S., and Stiner, M. (2007). Body ornamentation as information technology: towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In Mellars, P., Boyle, K., Bar-Yosef, O., and Stringer, C. (eds.), Rethinking the Human Revolution: New Behavioural and Biological Perspectives on the Origin and Dispersal of Modern Humans (pp. 4554). Cambridge: McDonald Institute Monographs.Google Scholar
Leakey, L. (1958). Recent discoveries at Olduvai Gorge, Tanganyika. Nature, 181, 10991103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Yélî Dnye and the theory of basic color terms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 10, 355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindly, J., and Clark, G. (1990). Symbolism and modern human origins. Current Anthropology, 31, 233–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marean, C., Bar-Matthews, M., Bernatchez, J., Fisher, E., Goldberg, P., Herries, A., Jacobs, Z., et al. (2007). Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature, 449, 905–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshack, A. (1976). Implications of the Paleolithic symbolic evidence for the origin of language. Current Anthropology, 17, 274–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshack, A. (1981). On Paleolithic ochre and early uses of color and symbol. Current Anthropology, 22, 188–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, R. (1996). Scaling of the mammalian brain: the maternal energy hypothesis. News in the Physiological Sciences, 11, 149–56.Google Scholar
Maynard-Smith, J., and Harper, D. (2003). Animal Signals. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McBrearty, S. (2003). Comment on Hovers et al. 2003. Current Anthropology, 44, 514.Google Scholar
McBrearty, S., and Brooks, A. (2000). The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. Journal of Human Evolution, 39, 453–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNabb, J., and Beaumont, P. (2011). A Report on the Archaeological Assemblages from Excavations by Peter Beaumont at Canteen Koppie, Northern Cape, South Africa. Oxford: BAR International Series 2275.Google Scholar
Mellars, P. (1996). The Neanderthal Legacy. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. (2000). The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (1999). Symbolism and the supernatural. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C., and Power, C. (eds.), The Evolution of Culture (pp. 147–69). Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. (2014). The cognition of Homo neanderthalensis and H. sapiens: does the use of pigment necessarily imply symbolic thought? In Akazawa, T. et al. (eds.), Dynamics of Learning in Neanderthals and Modern Humans, vol. II: Cognitive and Physical Perspectives (pp. 716). Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series. Tokyo: Springer Japan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Møller, A. (1997). Evolutionary conflicts and adapted psychologies. In CIBA Foundation Symposium, 208: Characterizing Human Psychological Adaptations (pp. 3950). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Negro, J., Margalida, A., Hiraldo, F., and Heredia, R. (1999). The function of the cosmetic coloration of bearded vultures: when art imitates life. Animal Behaviour, 58, F1417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oakley, K. (1981). Emergence of higher thought 3.0–0.2 Ma B.P. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 292, 205–11.Google Scholar
Paddayya, K. (1976). Excavation of an Acheulian site at Hunsgi, South India. Current Anthropology, 17, 760–1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. (1955). Logic as semiotic: the theory of signs. In Buchler, J. (ed.), The Philosophical Writings of Peirce (pp. 98119). New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Pettitt, P. (2011). The living as symbols, the dead as symbols: problematising the scale and pace of hominin symbolic evolution. In Henshilwood, C. and d’Errico, F. (eds.), Homo symbolicus: The Dawn of Language, Imagination and Spirituality (pp. 141–62). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Pfeiffer, J. (1982). The Creative Explosion. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Powell, A., Shennan, S., and Thomas, M. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324, 12981301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Power, C. (1999). ‘Beauty magic’: the origins of art. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C., and Power, C. (eds.), The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View (pp. 113–46). Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Power, C. (2009). Sexual selection models for the evolution of language: why they should be reversed. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The Cradle of Language (pp. 257–80). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Power, C., and Aiello, L. (1997). Female proto-symbolic strategies. In Hager, L. (ed.), Women in Human Evolution (pp. 153–71). New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Power, C., Somer, V., and Watts, I. (2013). The seasonality thermostat: female reproductive synchrony and male behavior in monkeys, Neanderthals, and modern humans. PaleoAnthropology, 2013, 3360.Google Scholar
Power, C., and Watts, I. (1997). The woman with the zebra’s penis: gender, mutability and performance. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute (N.S.), 3, 537–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rappaport, R. (1999). Ritual and Religion in the Making of Humanity. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Renfrew, C. (2008). Neuroscience, evolution and the sapient paradox: the factuality of value and of the sacred. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363, 2041–7.Google ScholarPubMed
Rifkin, R. (2011). Assessing the efficacy of red ochre as a prehistoric hide-tanning ingredient. Journal of African Archaeology, 9, 131–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rifkin, R. (2012). Processing ochre in the Middle Stone Age: testing the inference of prehistoric behaviours from actualistically derived experimental data. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 31, 174–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, R., and Jones, R. (1992). Luminescence dating of sediments: new light on the human colonization of Australia. Australian Aboriginal Studies, 2, 217.Google Scholar
Roebroeks, W., and Villa, P. (2011). On the earliest evidence for habitual use of fire in Europe. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 5209–14.Google ScholarPubMed
Rossano, M. (2010). Making friends, making tools, and making symbols. Current Anthropology, 51(Suppl. 1), S8998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossano, M. (2013). Mortal Rituals: What the Story of the Andes Survivors Tells Us About Human Evolution. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Šajnerová-Duškova, A., et al. (2009). Pitted and grinding stones from Middle Palaeolithic settlements in Bohemia: a functional study. In Sternke, F., Costa, L., and Eigeland, L. (eds.), Non-Flint Raw Material Use in Prehistory: Old Prejudices and New Directions (pp. 145–51). Proceedings of the XV Congress of the UISPp. Oxford: Archaeopress.Google Scholar
Salazar, D., Jackson, D., Guendon, J., Salinas, H., Morate, D., Figueroa, V., Manriquez, G., et al. (2011). Early evidence (ca. 12,000 BP) for iron oxide mining on the Pacific coast of South America. Current Anthropology, 52, 463–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salomon, H., Vignaud, C., Coquinot, Y., Beck, L., Stringer, C., Strivay, D., and d’Errico, F. (2012). Selection and heating of colouring materials in the Mousterian level of Es-Skhul (c. 100 000 years BP, Mount Carmel, Israel). Archaeometry, 54, 698722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The Construction of Social Reality. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Shillington, K. (2011). Luka Jantjie: Resistance Hero of the South African Frontier. Johannesburg: Witwatersrand University Press.Google Scholar
Shimelmitz, R., Kuhn, S., Jelinek, A., Ronen, A., Clark, A., Weinstein-Evron, M. (2014). “Fire at will”: The emergence of habitual fire use 350,000 years ago. Journal of Human Evolution, 77, 196203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skov, M., and Vartanian, O. (eds.) (2009). Neuroaesthetics. Amityville, NY: Baywood.Google Scholar
Soressi, M., and d’Errico, F. (2007). Pigments, gravures, parures: les comportements symboliques controversés des Néandertaliens. In Vandermeersch, B. and Maureille, B. (eds.), Les Néandertaliens, biologie et cultures (pp. 297309). Documents Préhistoriques, vol. XXIII. Paris: CTHS.Google Scholar
Sosis, R., and Alcorta, C. (2003). Signaling, solidarity and the sacred: the evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 264–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (1975). Rethinking Symbolism. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2011). From hominins to humans: how sapiens became behaviourally modern. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 366, 809–22.Google ScholarPubMed
Stiner, M., Gopher, A., and Barkai, R. (2011). Hearthside socioeconomics, hunting and paleoecology during the late Lower Paleolithic at Qesem Cave, Israel. Journal of Human Evolution, 60, 213–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stringer, C. (2011). The Origin of Our Species. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Stringer, C. (2012). The status of Homo heidelbergensis (Schoetensack 1908). Evolutionary Anthropology, 21, 101–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., and Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53, 673–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds? SubStance, 94/95, 627.Google Scholar
Underhill, D. (2011). The study of the Fauresmith: a review. South African Archaeological Bulletin, 66, 1526.Google Scholar
van Peer, P., Rots, V., and Vroomans, J.-M. (2004). A story of colorful diggers and grinders: the Sangoan and Lupemban at site 8-B-11, Sai Island, Northern Sudan. Before Farming, 3, 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C., Isler, K., and Burkart, J. (2012). Explaining brain size variation: from social to cultural brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 277–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voland, E. (2003). Aesthetic preferences in the world of artifacts – adaptations for the evaluation of ‘honest signals’? In Voland and Grammer, Evolutionary Aesthetics (pp. 239–60).Google Scholar
Voland, E., and Grammer, K. (eds.) (2003). Evolutionary Aesthetics. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadley, L., Hodgskiss, T., and Grant, M. (2009). Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 9590–4.Google ScholarPubMed
Walker, D., and Vul, E. (2014). Hierarchical encoding makes individuals in a group seem more attractive. Psychological Science, 25, 230–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, I. (1999). The origin of symbolic culture. In Dunbar, R., Knight, C., and Power, C. (eds.), The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View (pp. 113–46). Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Watts, I. (2002). Ochre in the Middle Stone Age of southern Africa: ritualized display or hide preservative? South African Archaeological Bulletin, 57, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, I. (2005). ‘Time, too, grows on the Moon’: some evidence for Knight’s theory of a human universal. In James, W. and Mills, D. (eds.), The Qualities of Time: Anthropological Perspectives (pp. 95118). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Watts, I. (2009). Red ochre, body painting and language: interpreting the Blombos ochre. In Botha, R. and Knight, C. (eds.), The Cradle of Language (pp. 6292). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watts, I. (2010). The pigments from Pinnacle Point Cave 13B, Western Cape, South Africa. Journal of Human Evolution, 59, 392411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watts, I. (2014). The red thread: pigment use and the evolution of collective ritual. In Dor, D., Lewis, J., and Knight, C. (eds.), The Social Evolution of Language (pp. 208–27). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Watts, I., Chazan, I., and Wilkins, J. (in press). Early evidence for brilliant ritual display: specularite use in the Northern Cape (South Africa) between ~500 ka and ~300 ka. Current Anthropology.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of Religiosity: A Cognitive Theory of Religious Transmission. Walnut Creek, CA: Altamira.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2002). Modes of religiosity: Towards a cognitive explanation of the sociopolitical dynamics of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion, 14, 219315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiten, A., and Erdal, D. (2012). The human socio-cognitive niche and its evolutionary origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 367, 2119–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Whiten, A., and van Schaik, C. (2007). The evolution of animal cultures and social intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 362, 602–20.Google ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, J., and Chazan, M. (2012). Blade production ~500 thousand years ago at Kathu Pan 1, South Africa: support for a multiple origins hypothesis for early Middle Pleistocene blade technologies. Journal of Archaeological Science, 39, 1883–1900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, J., Schoville, B., Brown, K., and Chazan, M. (2012). Evidence for early hafted hunting technology. Science, 338, 942–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wreschner, E. (1985). Evidence and interpretation of red ochre in the early prehistoric sequences. In Tobias, P. (ed.), Hominid Evolution: Past, Present and Future (pp. 387–94). New York: Alan Liss.Google Scholar
Wynn, T., and Coolidge, F. (2012). How to Think like a Neandertal. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., and Zahavi, A. (1997). The Handicap Principle. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaidel, D. (2010). Art and brain: insights from neuropsychology, biology and evolution. Journal of Anatomy, 216, 177–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: an archaeological perspective on the origins of “behavioural modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15, 154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilhão, J. (2011). The emergence of language, art and symbolic thinking: a Neanderthal test of competing hypotheses. In Henshilwood, C. and d’Errico, F. (eds.), Homo symbolicus: The Dawn of Language, Imagination and Spirituality (pp. 111–32). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×