Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T11:09:18.636Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Role of Self-Regulation in the Transition to School

from Part II - Neurobiological and Ecological Markers

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2023

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Sonali Nag
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

This chapter goes into the role of children’s self-regulation in their transition from home to school. Over the last two decades, a sizable body of research has documented the importance of the early childhood years as a critical foundation not only for a successful transition to school, but for literacy success in elementary school and beyond. There is evidence that a complex set of factors in the child, family, school, and larger sociocultural context, independently and in interaction, shape the growth of early literacy skills over that crucial time period. Recently, attention has focused on a set of skills called self-regulation (also known as executive function or effortful control), which has been shown to uniquely impact children’s literacy development and academic growth across the school years, as well as success in adult life. This chapter focuses on how self-regulation can be conceptualized. In addition, it examines the extent and nature of individual differences in self-regulation during the transition to school and what unique impact it has on early literacy and later academic achievement. Finally, it is explored to what extent self-regulation can be modified through appropriate environmental stimulation in the home and school environment.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahmed, S. F., Grammer, J., & Morrison, F. (2021). Cognition in context: Validating group-based executive function assessments in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 208, 105131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, C., & Razza, R. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78, 647663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brod, G., Bunge, S. A., & Shing, Y. L. (2017). Does one year of schooling improve children’s cognitive control and alter associated brain function? Psychological Science. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617699838.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronson, M. B., Tivnan, T., & Seppanen, P. S. (1995). Relations between teacher and classroom activity variables and the classroom behaviors of preschool children in Chapter 1 funded programs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 253282. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016=0193-3973(95)90035-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burrage, M., Ponitz, C. C., Shah, P. et al. (2008). A natural experiment of schooling effects on executive functions. Child Neuropsychology, 14(6), 510524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, C. E., Connor, C. M., & Morrison, F .J. (2005) Effects of variation in teacher organization on classroom function. Journal of School Psychology, 43, 6185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christian, K., Morrison, F. J., Frazier, J. A., & Massetti, G. (2000). Specificity in the nature and timing of cognitive growth in kindergarten and first grade. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 429448. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0104_04.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., & Germeroth, C. (2016). Learning executive function and early mathematics: Directions of causal relations. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36, 7990.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, C. M., Ponitz, C. E. C., Phillips, B. et al. (2010). First graders’ literacy and self-regulation gains: The effect of individualizing instruction. Journal of School Psychology, 48, 433455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4–12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959964. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duckworth, A., & Seligman, M. P. (2006). Self-discipline gives girls the edge: Gender in self-discipline, grades, and achievement test scores. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 198208. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Liu, Y., Orr, J. M., & Carp, J. (2012). The error-related negativity (ERN/Ne). In Luck, S. J. & Kappenman, E. S. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components (pp. 231291). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grammer, J. K., Carrasco, M., Gehring, W. J., & Morrison, F. J. (2014). Age-related changes in error processing in young children: A school-based investigation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 93105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grammer, J. K., Gehring, W. J., & Morrison, F. J. (2018). Associations between developmental changes in error‐related brain activity and executive functions in early childhood. Psychophysiology, 55(3), e13040.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hackman, D. A., & Farah, M. J. (2009). Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(2), 6573. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hillman, C. H., Pontifex, M. B., Motl, R. W. et al. (2012). From ERPs to academics. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2 (Supplement 1), S90S98. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.07.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh, J. B., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Error-related negativity predicts academic performance. Psychophysiology, 47, 192196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imbens, G. W., & Lemieux, T. (2008). Regression discontinuity designs: A guide to practice. Journal of Econometrics, 142, 615635. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeconom.2007.05.001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, R., Zhu, P., Somers, M., & Bloom, H. (2012). A practical guide to regression discontinuity (Working Paper). New York: MDRC.Google Scholar
Jaramillo, J. M., Rendon, M. I., Munoz, L., Weis, M., & Trommsdorff, G. (2017). Children’s self-regulation in cultural contexts: The role of parental socialization theories, goals, and practices. Frontiers in Psychology (June 6) (8), 923. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00923.PMID:28634460;PMCID:PMC5460587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M. H., Bousselot, T., & Ahmed, S. F. (2021). Executive functions and science achievement during the five-to-seven-year shift. Developmental Psychology, 57(12), 21192133. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0001261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M. H., Grammer, J. K., Marulis, L. M. et al. (2016). Early math and reading achievement are associated with the error positivity. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 1826.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M. H., Marulis, L. M., Grammer, J. K., Morrison, F. J., & Gehring, W. J. (2017). Motivational processes from expectancy-value theory are associated with variability in the error positivity in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 3247. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.10.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, M. H., Morrison, F. J. (2018). Schooling effects on literacy skills during the transition to school. AERA Open, 4(3), 115. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858418798793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamm, C., Zelazo, P. D., & Lewis, M. D. (2006). Neural correlates of cognitive control in childhood and adolescence: disentangling the contributions of age and executive function. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 21392148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, J. S., Ponitz, C., & Morrison, F. J. (2009). Early gender differences in self-regulation and academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 689704. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, M. M., Acock, A. C., & Morrison, F. J. (2006). The impact of kindergarten learning-related skills on academic trajectories at the end of elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 471490.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D. et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS Proceedings Of The National Academy Of Sciences Of The United States Of America, 108, 26932698. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrison, F. J., Bachman, H. J., & Connor, C. M. (2005). Improving Literacy in America: Guidelines from Research. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, F. J., & Grammer, J. K. (2016). Conceptual clutter and measurement mayhem: Proposals for cross disciplinary integration in conceptualizing and measuring executive function. In Griffin, J., McCardle, P., & Freund, L. (eds.), Research Directions in Preschool Executive Functions: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment and Translational Research. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Morrison, F. J., Griffith, E. M., & Frazier, J. A. (1996). Schooling and the 5 to 7 shift: A natural experiment. In Sameroff, A. J. & Haith, M. M. (eds.), The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility (pp. 161–186). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Morrison, F. J., Kim, M. H., Connor, C. M., & Grammer, J. K. (2019). The causal impact of schooling on children’s development: Lessons for developmental science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(5), 441449. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419855661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, F. J., Ponitz, C. C., & McClelland, M. M. (2010). Self-regulation and academic achievement in the transition to school. In Calkins, S. and Bell, M. (eds.), Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition (pp. 203224). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moser, J. S., Schroder, H. S., Heeter, C., Moran, T. P., & Lee, Y-H. (2011). Mind your errors: Evidence for a neural mechanism linking growth mind-set to adaptive posterror adjustments. Psychological Science, 22(12), 14841489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noble, K. G., Norman, M. F., & Farah, M. J. (2005). Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science, 8(1), 7487. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00394.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
OECD. (2020). Early Learning and Child Well-Being: A Study of Five-year-Olds in England, Estonia, and the United States. Paris: OECD Publishing. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1787/3990407f-en.Google Scholar
Oh, S., & Lewis, C. (2008). Korean preschoolers’ advanced inhibitory control and its relation to other executive skills and mental state understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 8099. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01112.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Overbeek, T. J. M., Nieuwenhuis, S., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Dissociable components of error processing: On the functional significance of the Pe vis-à-vis the ERN/Ne. Journal of Psychophysiology, 19(4), 319329. https://doi.org/10.1027/0269-8803.19.4.319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, P., & Kievit, R. A. (2020). The development of academic achievement and cognitive abilities: A bidirectional perspective. Child Development Perspectives, 14(1), 1520. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabbagh, M. A., Xu, F., Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Lee, K. (2006). The development of executive functioning and theory of mind: A comparison of Chinese and US preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17(1), 7481. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01667.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skibbe, L. E., Connor, C. M., Morrison, F. J., & Jewkes, A. M. (2011). Schooling effects on preschoolers’ self-regulation, early literacy, and language growth. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 4249. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.05.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thistlethwaite, D. L., & Campbell, D. T. (1960). Regression-discontinuity analysis: An alternative to the ex post facto experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51(6), 309317. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobin, J. J., Hsueh, Y., & Karasawa, M. (2009). Preschool in Three Cultures Revisited: China, Japan, and the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V., Grasman, R. P. P. P. et al. (2006). A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842861.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X. et al. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: The US, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 621633. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.04.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiland, C., & Yoshikawa, H. (2013). Impacts of a prekindergarten program on children’s mathematics, language, literacy, executive function, and emotional skills. Child Development, 84(6), 2112–30. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weixler, L. B. (2012). The contributions of preschool attendance and kindergarten experience to executive functioning in Chinese and American children. (PhD, University of Michigan.) https://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/handle/2027.42/96108.Google Scholar
Welsh, M. C. (2001). The prefrontal cortex and the development of executive functions. In Kalverboer, A. & Gramsbergen, A. (eds.), Handbook of Brain and Behaviour Development (pp. 767789). Deventer: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Welsh, M. C., Friedman, S. L., & Spieker, S. J. (2006). Executive functions in developing children: Current conceptualizations and questions for the future. In McCartney, K., Phillips, D., McCartney, K., & Phillips, D. (eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development (pp. 167187). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, V. C., Cook, T. D., Barnett, W. S., & Jung, K. (2008). An effectiveness-based evaluation of five state pre-kindergarten programs. Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 27(1), 122154. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/pam.20310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL:Riverside.Google Scholar
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(03), 412422. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728910000611.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zelazo, P., Craik, F. M., & Booth, L. (2004). Executive function across the life span. Acta Psychologica, 115, 167183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×