Published online by Cambridge University Press: 22 September 2009
Throughout the past century biopsychosocial models of adolescent development have increased in both their promotion and prevalence (reviewed by Susman, 1997). The biological process of puberty is the typical initiator of the adolescent period of human development (Petersen, 1985). Thus, it is not surprising that the majority of adolescent biopsychosocial research has considered pubertal timing or status (Graber, Petersen, and Brooks-Gunn, 1996) as predictors of social (e.g., Meschke and Silbereisen, 1997; Meschke, et al., 2001) and psychological outcomes (Susman, et al., 1985).
The timing of pubertal onset has potentially negative implications for subsequent psychological and physical health (see chapter 12). Research indicates that early onset of puberty is associated with an increased likelihood of adolescent problem behavior (Caspi and Moffitt, 1991; Steinberg, 1989), depression, anxiety (Brooks-Gunn, 1988; Susman, et al., 1985), and breast cancer in women (Kampert, Whittemore, and Paffenbarger, 1988). The potential health consequences of early reproductive maturation have motivated researchers to investigate the possible predictors of pubertal timing.
Recently, environmental factors have been related to pubertal development (reviewed by Kim, Smith, and Palermiti, 1997). Stress is the environmental factor of interest and has typically been divided into two categories of predictors: physical stressors and psychosocial stressors. Increased physical stressors (e.g., decreased nutritional intake or increased exercise; Brooks-Gunn and Warren, 1985; Frisch, 1983) are more often associated with delayed pubertal timing. Psychosocial stressors (e.g., family conflict or depressed mood; Brooks-Gunn and Warren, 1985; Frisch, 1983; Graber, Brooks-Gunn, and Warren, 1995) are often associated with accelerated pubertal timing.
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.