Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T16:25:42.679Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Plasticity and Variation: Cultural Influences on Parenting and Early Child Development Within and Across Populations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2010

Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Paul M. Plotsky
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Daniel S. Schechter
Affiliation:
Hôpitaux Universitaires de Genève
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
Foundation for Psychocultural Research, California
Get access

Summary

This chapter examines two moments in the twentieth century when anthropology made a major contribution to developmental research or was in a position to do so. I seek to clarify what progress has been made up to now and discern what can be done to find a way forward.

The question of whether anthropology can make a central contribution to our understanding of human development has been asked – and answered affirmatively – at least since Margaret Mead (1928, 1930, 1931) raised it in the first third of the 20th century, and numerous times since then by the Whitings (e.g., J. Whiting, 1954; Whiting & Whiting, 1960) and others, including several contributors to this volume. Forceful arguments, vivid illustrations, and empirical evidence have been assembled – repeatedly, recently as well as in the past, and in interdisciplinary forums – to argue the necessity of cross-cultural research on childhood environments and development for a science of human child development and, more specifically, for developmental psychology and psychiatry. Is there any need to make this case again?

Apparently so. All the evidence I am aware of suggests that even in this first decade of the 21st century, with the exception of nutritional science, anthropological, and other cross-cultural studies remain marginal to and of minor significance in the mainstreams of the research disciplines investigating child development (developmental psychology, child psychiatry, pediatrics, and education).

Type
Chapter
Information
Formative Experiences
The Interaction of Caregiving, Culture, and Developmental Psychobiology
, pp. 11 - 35
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Bateson, G., & Mead, M. (1942). Balinese character: A photographic analysis. New York: New York Academy of Sciences.Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1892). The growth of children. Science, 19, 256–257, 281–282; 20, 351–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boas, F. (1902). Rudolf Virchow's anthropological work. Science, 16, 441–445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boas, F. (1912a). Changes in bodily form of descendants of immigrants. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1912b). Changes in the bodily form of descendants of immigrants. American Anthropologist, 14, 530–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boas, F. (1912c). Instability of human types. In Spiller, G. (Ed.), Papers on interracial problems communicated to the first Universal Races Congress held at the University of London, July 26–29, 1911 (pp. 99–103). Boston: Ginn & Co.Google Scholar
Boas, F., Ed. (1938). General anthropology. Boston: D.C. Heath & Co.Google Scholar
Bogin, B. (1999). Patterns of human growth (2nd ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Bogin, B. (2001). The growth of humanity. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
Bogin, B., & Loucky, J. (1997). Plasticity, political economy, and physical growth status of Guatemala Maya children living in the United States. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 102, 17–32.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Caudill, W. (1949). Psychological characteristics of acculturated Wisconsin Ojibwa children. American Anthropologist, 51, 409–427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caudill, W. (1961). Around the clock patient care in Japanese psychiatric hospitals: The role of the Tsukisoi. American Sociological Review, 26, 204–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caudill, W. (1963). Sibling rank and style of life among Japanese psychiatric patients. In Akimoto, H. (Ed.), Proceedings of the Joint Meeting of the Japanese Society of Psychiatry and Neurology and the American Psychiatric Association, May 13–17, 1963, Tokyo, Japan.Google Scholar
Caudill, W., & Plath, D. (1966). Who sleeps by whom? Parent-child involvement in urban Japanese families. Psychiatry, 29, 344–366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caudill, W., & Schooler, C. (1973). Child behavior and child rearing in Japan and the United States: An interim report. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157, 323–338.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caudill, W., & Weinstein, H. (1969). Maternal care and infant behavior in Japan and America. Psychiatry, 32, 12–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darnell, R. (1986). Personality and culture: The fate of the Sapirian alternative. In Stocking, G. (Ed.), Malinowski, Rivers, Benedict and others: Essays on culture and personality (pp. 156–183). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Darnell, R. (1990). Edward Sapir: Linguist, anthropologist, humanist. Berkeley and Los Angeles: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Doi, T. (1973a). The anatomy of dependence. New York: Kodansha International.Google Scholar
Doi, T. (1973b). Omote and ura: Concepts derived from the Japanese two-fold structure of consciousness. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157, 258–261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gravelee, C., Bernard, H. R., & Leonard, W. R. (2003). Heredity, environment, and cranial form: A reanalysis of Boas's immigrant data. American Anthropologist, 105, 125–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heim, C., Plotsky, P., & Nemeroff, C. (2004). Importance of studying the contributions of early adverse experience to neurobiological findings in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 29, 641–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschner, S. (1996). Religious and romantic origins of psychoanalysis: Individuation and integration in post-Freudian theory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lebra, T. S. (1976). Patterns of Japanese behavior. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Lebra, T. S. (1994). Mother and child in Japanese socialization: A Japan–U.S. comparison. In Greenfield, P. & Cocking, R. (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 259–274). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
LeVine, R. A., & Norman, K. (2001). The infant's acquisition of culture: Early attachment reexamined in anthropological perspective. In Moore, C. & Mathews, H. (Eds.), The psychology of cultural experience (pp. 83–104). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mahler, M. (1958). On human symbiosis and the vicissitudes of individuation. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Mahler, M., Pine, F., & Bergman, A. (1975). The psychological birth of the human infant: Symbiosis and individuation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1930). Growing up in New Guinea. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1931). Research on primitive children. In Murchison, C. (Ed.), Manual of child psychology. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press.Google Scholar
Pierpont, C. (2004, March 8). The measure of America: How a rebel anthropologist waged war on racism. The New Yorker, pp. 48–63.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, F., Pott, M., & Azuma, H. (2000). The development of close relationships in Japan and the United States: Paths of symbolic harmony and generative tension. Child Development, 71, 1121–1142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanchez, M., Ladd, C., & Plotsky, P. (2001). Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 419–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapir, E. (1993). The psychology of culture: A course of lectures. Reconstructed and edited by J. T. Irvine. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Schooler, C. (1996). William Caudill and the reproduction of culture: Infant, child and maternal behavior in Japan and the United States. In Schwalb, D. & Schwalb, B. (Eds.), Japanese childrearing: Two generations of scholarship. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schooler, C., & Caudill, W. (1964). Symptomatology in Japanese and American schizophrenics. Ethnology, 3, 172–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shonkoff, J., & Phillips, D. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. Washington: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Sparks, C., & Jantz, R. (2002). A reassessment of human cranial plasticity: Boas revisited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(23), pp. 14636–14639. doi: 10.1073/pnas.222389599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stocking, G. W. (1968). Race, culture and evolution: Essays in the history of anthropology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Stocking, G. (1974). A Franz Boas reader: The shaping of American anthropology (pp. 1883–1911). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stocking, G. (1992). Polarity and plurality: Franz Boas as psychological anthropologist. In Schwartz, T., White, G., & Lutz, C. (Eds.), New directions in psychological anthropology (pp. 311–323). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. M. (1959). Boas' contribution to knowledge of human growth and form. In Goldschmidt, W. (Ed.) The anthropology of Franz Boas. Memoir No. 89 of the American Anthropological Association, 61, 76–111.Google Scholar
Vogel, E., & Schooler, C. (Eds.) (1973). In memory of William A. Caudill, Pts. I and II. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 157(4, 5), 231–395.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. (2001). The American dependency conflict: Continuities and discontinuities in behavior and values of countercultural parents and their children. Ethos, 29, 271–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiting, J. (1954). The cross-cultural method. In Lindzey, G. (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 523–531). Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Whiting, J., & Whiting, B. (1960). Contributions of anthropology to methods of studying child rearing. In Mussen, P. (Ed.), Handbook of research methods in child development (pp. 918–944). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×