Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T12:21:10.995Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Thinking the Unthinkable: Cognitive Appraisals and Hurt Feelings

from PART II - CONCEPTUALIZING HURT

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Anita L. Vangelisti
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

“A man is hurt not so much by what happens to him as by his opinion of what happens.”

Montaigne, 16th century

As a rule, human beings neither seek nor enjoy the experience of pain. By definition, pain involves suffering, and given the choice, we would rather go through life without it. Fortunately, we have no such choice – fortunately, because pain is critical to our survival. Without the felt experience of pain, we would fail to notice injuries. Only the smell of burning flesh would alert us to the fact that we were on fire, and we would have no inkling that our leg was shattered until we noticed that walking was difficult. Pain, then, is adaptive and helps keep us alive. However, pain is not just a physical phenomenon; it is also psychological. Experiences of heartache and loneliness involve psychological suffering, and this too is adaptive. As social creatures, humans are dependent on their relationships with others – in particular, parents, siblings, partners, and friends – for their survival. Just as physical pain alerts individuals to physical harm, so too does psychological pain alert them to social harm such as rejection or abandonment (MacDonald & Leary, 2005). The pain of appendicitis and the pain of social loss, then, serve similar functions: they let us know we are in trouble, they motivate us to cry out in protest, and they motivate others to come to our aid.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersen, S. M., & Chen, S. (2002). The relational self: An interpersonal social-cognitive theory. Psychological Review, 109, 619–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, M. (1960). Emotion and personality: Vol. 1. Psychological aspects. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation, anxiety, and anger. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Loss, sadness, and depression. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Downey, G., & Feldman, , , S. I. (1996). Implications of rejection sensitivity for intimate relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327–1343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Downey, G., Freitas, A. L., Michaelis, B., & Khouri, H. (1998). The self-fulfilling prophecy in close relationships: Rejection sensitivity and rejection by romantic partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 545–560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellsworth, P. (1991). Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories of emotion. In Strongman, K. T. (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 143–162). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P. (2007). Appraisals, emotions, and adaptation. In Forgas, J. P., Haselton, M., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind (pp. 71–88). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ellsworth, P., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Goldsmith, H., & Scherer, K. R. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572–595). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Feeney, J. A. (2005). Hurt feelings in couple relationships: Exploring the role of attachment and perceptions of personal injury. Personal Relationships, 12, 253–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitness, J. (2001). Betrayal, rejection, revenge, and forgiveness: An interpersonal script approach. In Leary, M. (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 73–104). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fitness, J., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). Love, hate, anger, and jealousy in close relationships: A prototype and cognitive appraisal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 942–958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitness, J., & Fletcher, G. J. O., & Overall, N. (2003). Interpersonal attraction and intimate relationships. In Cooper, J. & Hogg, M. (Eds.), Sage handbook of social psychology (pp. 258–278). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (2007). The laws of emotion. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kappas, A. (2006). Appraisals are direct, immediate, intuitive, and unwitting and some are reflective. Emotion, 20, 952–975.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740–752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In Zanna, M. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 32, pp. 1–62). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. A. (2001). Hurt feelings: The neglected emotion. In Kowalski, R. (Ed.), Behaving badly: Aversive behaviors in interpersonal relationships (pp. 151–175). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., Springer, C., Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). The causes, phenomenology and consequences of hurt feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1225–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 202–223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Attachment theory and emotions in close relationships: Exploring the attachment-related dynamics of emotional reactions to relational events. Personal Relationships, 12, 149–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Collins, N. L. (2006). Optimizing assurance: The risk regulation system in relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 641–666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parkinson, B. (1997). Untangling the appraisal-emotion connection. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1, 62–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Planalp, S., & Fitness, J. (1999). Thinking/feeling about social and personal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16, 731–750.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Canyas, R., & Downey, G. (2005). Rejection sensitivity as a predictor of affective and behavioral responses to interpersonal stress. In Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 131–154). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: A structural theory. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Roseman, I. (2004). Appraisals, rather than unpleasantness or muscle movements, are the primary determinants of specific emotions. Emotion, 4, 145–150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. (1984). On the nature and function of emotion: A component process approach. In Scherer, K. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 293–318). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scherer, K. (1994). Evidence for both universality and cultural specificity of emotion elicitation. In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 172–175). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Collins, N., & Clark, C. L. (1996). Attachment styles and internal working models of self and relationship partners. In Fletcher, G. J. O. & Fitness, J. (Eds.), Knowledge structures in close relationships: A social psychological approach (pp. 25–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, C., & Ellsworth, P. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J., & Salovey, P. (1999). Problematic social emotions: Shame, guilt, jealousy and envy. In Kowalski, R. & Leary, M. (Eds.), The social psychology of emotional and behavioral problems (pp. 167–195). Washington: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, and consciousness, Vol. 2. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Tong, E. M. W., Bishop, G. D., Enkelmann, H. C., Why, Y. P., Diong, S. M., Khader, M., & Ang, J. (2007). Emotion and appraisal: A study using ecological momentary assessment. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 1361–1381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L. (2001). Making sense of hurtful interactions in close relationships. In Manusov, V. & Harvey, J. H. (Eds.), Attribution, communication behavior, and close relationships (pp. 38–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., & Young, S. L. (2000). When words hurt: The effects of perceived intentionality on interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 393–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., Young, S. L., Carpenter-Theune, K. E., & Alexander, A. L. (2005). Why does it hurt? The perceived causes of hurt feelings. Communication Research, 32, 443–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warburton, W. A., & Williams, K. D. (2005). Ostracism: When social motives collide. In Forgas, J. P., Williams, K. D., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes (pp. 294–313). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×