Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T11:59:14.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Cultural Influences on the Causes and Experience of Hurt Feelings

from PART V - HURT IN APPLIED CONTEXTS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Anita L. Vangelisti
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Austin
Get access

Summary

The purpose of this chapter is to examine cultural influences on the antecedents, phenomenology, and expression of hurt feelings. Most research on hurt feelings has been conducted in Western cultures, with relatively little attention paid to the ways in which the experience and expression of hurt feelings might vary cross-culturally. Furthermore, research on hurt feelings, relative to other emotions, is in its infancy. A key issue is the degree to which hurt feelings represent a distinct emotional state as opposed to the degree to which hurt feelings are simply a blend of other emotions. Although people who experience hurt feelings may also be sad, angry, or fearful – all emotions that have received fairly extensive attention from cross-cultural researchers – we cannot simply assume that findings from research on sadness, anger, and fear directly apply to hurt feelings (Vangelisti, Young, Carpenter-Theune, & Alexander, 2005). In addition, other emotions that often accompany hurt feelings are caused by other, different appraisals in the situation. If a rejection event that produces hurt feelings seems unjustified, a person might feel angry; if the rejection event involves a loss, he or she might feel sad. But these are separate from the appraisals that cause hurt per se (see Leary & Leder, Chapter 2, this volume). To the degree that cross-cultural research has been useful in helping us delineate emotions other than hurt feelings, particularly given differences in appraisals of some situations across, for example, individualistic and collectivist cultures, examining cultural influences on hurt feelings should aid us in advancing our knowledge of this underresearched emotion.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 3, 497–529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., & DeWall, C. N. (2005). The inner dimension of social exclusion: Intelligent thought and self-regulation among rejected persons. In Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 53–73). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bourgeois, K. S., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Coping with rejection: Derogating those who choose us last. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 101–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Attributions and behavior in marital interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 613–628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. (2003). Introduction: Emotion and health. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 1047–1052). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cocroft, B., & Ting-Toomey, S. (1994). Facework in Japan and the United States. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18, 469–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lama, Dalai. (2002). Understanding our fundamental nature. In Davidson, R. J. & Harrington, A. (Eds.), Visions of compassion (pp. 66–80). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, R. J., & Harrington, A. (2002). Visions of compassion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
DeWall, C. N., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Alone but feeling no pain: Effects of social exclusion on physical pain tolerance and pain threshold, affective forecasting, and interpersonal empathy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2005). The neurocognitive overlap between physical and social pain. In Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 109–127). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. J. (1994). Afterward: How is evidence of universals in antecedents of emotion explained? In Ekman, P. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion (pp. 176–177). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Feeney, J. A. (2005). Hurt feelings in couple relationships: Exploring the role of attachment and perceptions of personal injury. Personal Relationships, 12, 253–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fincham, F. D., Harold, G. T., & Gano-Phillips, S. (2000). The longitudinal association between attributions and marital satisfaction: Direction of effects and role of efficacy expectations. Journal of Family Psychology, 14, 267–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitness, J. (2001). Betrayal, rejection, revenge, and forgiveness: An interpersonal script approach. In Leary, M. R. (Ed.), Interpersonal rejection (pp. 73–103). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giese-Davis, J., & Spiegel, D. (2003). Emotional expression and cancer progression. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 1053–1082). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Hiding feelings: The acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 95–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heelas, P. (1996). Emotion talk across cultures. In Harre, R. & Parrott, W. G. (Eds.), The emotions (pp. 171–199). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Knowledge and appraisal in the cognition-emotion relationship. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 281–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 1–62). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Kowalski, R. M., Smith, L., & Phillips, S. (2003). Teasing, rejection, and violence: Case studies of the school shootings. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 202–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., & Springer, C. (2001). Hurt feelings: The neglected emotion. In Kowalski, R. M. (Ed.), Aversive interpersonal behaviors (pp. 151–175). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., Springer, C., Negel, L., Ansell, E., & Evans, K. (1998). The causes, phenomenology, and consequences of hurt feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1225–1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leary, M. R., Twenge, J. M., & Quinlivan, E. (2006). Interpersonal rejection as a determinant of anger and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 111–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebra, T. S. (1976). Japanese patterns of behavior. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.Google Scholar
Levy, R. I. (1984). The emotions in comparative perspective. In Scherer, K. R. & Ekman, P. (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 397–412). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacDonald, G., Kingsbury, R., & Shaw, S. (2005). Adding insult to injury: Social pain theory and response to social exclusion. In Williams, K. D., Forgas, J. P., & Hippel, W. (Eds.), The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying (pp. 77–90). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 202–223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, D., Kudoh, T., Scherer, K., & Wallbott, H. (1988). Antecedents of and reactions to emotions in the United States and Japan. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 19, 267–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauro, R., Sato, K., & Tucker, J. (1992). The role of appraisal in human emotions: A cross-cultural study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 301–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesquita, B. (2003). Emotions as dynamic cultural phenomena. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 871–890). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Ellsworth, P. (2001). The role of culture in appraisal. In Scherer, K. R. & Schorr, A. (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 233–248). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 179–204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesquita, B., Frijda, N. H., & Scherer, K. R. (1997). Culture and emotion. In Dasen, P. & Saraswathi, T. S. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 255–297). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Markus, H. R. (2004). Culture and emotion: Models of agency as sources of cultural variation in emotion. In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam symposium (pp. 341–358). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Walker, R. (2003). Cultural differences in emotions: A context for interpreting emotional experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 777–793.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, D. W., & Baumgarte, R. (2004). Cross-cultural misunderstandings reduce empathic responding. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34, 391–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K., & Duncan, E. (1992). Incidents of emotion in daily life. In Strongman, K. T. (Ed.), International review of studies on emotion (Vol. 2, pp. 249–293). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Planalp, S. (1999). Communicating emotion: Social, moral, and cultural processes. Paris: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R. (1997). Profiles of emotion-antecedent appraisal: Testing theoretical predictions across cultures. Cognition & Emotion, 11, 113–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1988). Emotional experience in cultural context: A comparison between Europe, Japan, and the United States. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Faces of emotions (pp. 5–30). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Shaver, P. R., Wu, S., & Schwartz, J. C. (1992). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation: A prototype approach. In Clark, M. S. (Ed.), Emotion (pp. 175–212). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Smith, C. A. (1991). The self, appraisal, and coping. In Snyder, C. R. & Forsyth, D. R. (Eds.), Handbook of social and clinical psychology (pp. 116–137). New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Snapp, C. M., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Hurt feelings among new acquaintances: Moderating effects of interpersonal familiarity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18, 315–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snyder, C. R., & Lopez, S. J. (2007). Positive psychology: The scientific and practical explorations of human strengths. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Stephan, W. G., Stephan, C. W., & Vargas, M. (1996). Emotional expression in Costa Rica and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 147–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, C. W., Stephan, W. G., Saito, I., & Barnett, S. M. (1998). Emotional expression in Japan and the United States: The nonmonolithic nature of individualism and collectivism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 728–748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ting-Toomey, S., Gao, G., Trubisky, P., Yang, Z., Kim, H. S., Lin, S. L., & Nishida, T. (1991). Culture, face maintenance, and styles of handling interpersonal conflict: A study in five cultures. International Journal of Conflict Management, 2, 275–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L. (1994). Messages that hurt. In Cupach, W. R. & Spitzberg, B. H. (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 53–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., & Crumley, L. P. (1998). Reactions to messages that hurt: The influence of relational contexts. Communication Monographs, 65, 173–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., & Young, S. L. (2000). When words hurt: The effects of perceived intentionality on interpersonal relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17, 393–424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vangelisti, A. L., Young, S. L., Carpenter-Theune, K. E., & Alexander, A. L. (2005). Why does it hurt? The perceived causes of hurt feelings. Communication Research, 32, 443–477.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallbott, H. G., & Scherer, K. (1988). How universal and specific is emotional experience? Evidence from 27 countries and five continents. In Scherer, K. R. (Ed.), Facets of emotion (pp. 31–56). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Whitesell, N. R., & Harter, S. (1996). The interpersonal context of emotion: Anger with close friends and classmates. Child Development, 67, 1345–1359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wong, P. T. P., & Weiner, B. (1981). When people ask “why” questions, and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 650–663.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, S. L., & Bippus, A. M. (2001). Does it make a difference if they hurt you in a funny way? Humorously and non-humorously phrased hurtful messages in personal relationships. Communication Quarterly, 49, 35–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×