Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T04:33:52.841Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Pavlovian Conditioning, Survival, and Reproductive Success

from Part I - Evolution of Learning Processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2022

Mark A. Krause
Affiliation:
Southern Oregon University
Karen L. Hollis
Affiliation:
Mount Holyoke College, Massachusetts
Mauricio R. Papini
Affiliation:
Texas Christian University
Get access

Summary

Learning enables organisms to make adaptive modifications to ecological circumstances. Pavlovian conditioning is a specific form of learning that involves learning about predictive relations between events in the environment and enables animals to form associations that facilitate adaptive modifications in behavior in both appetitive and aversive contexts. Pavlovian conditioned stimuli are functionally important because they prepare organisms to interact with biologically relevant stimuli, such as potential mates, and facilitate how those interactions occur. Research with various species indicates that Pavlovian conditioning influences physiological responses that affect reproductive success. In this chapter, we review how Pavlovian conditioning results in enhanced reproductive physiological responses, increased success in fertilization, and increased numbers of offspring produced. Research has shown that animals that had a chance to learn about mating opportunities have a distinct reproductive advantage over those that did not and therefore are more likely to contribute their genes to future generations. This research informs our understanding of how Pavlovian learning is not just a proximate mechanism of behavior, but also has a role in genetic transmission and thereby contributes to the future course of evolution.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adkins-Regan, E. (1999). Foam produced by male Coturnix quail: What is its function? The Auk, 116(1), 184193. https://doi.org/10.2307/4089465Google Scholar
Adkins-Regan, E., & MacKillop, E. A. (2003). Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) inseminations are more likely to fertilize eggs in a context predicting mating opportunities. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 270, 16851689. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2003.2421CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akins, C. K. (1998). Context excitation and modulation of conditioned sexual behavior. Animal Learning & Behavior, 26, 416426. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03199234Google Scholar
Akins, C. K. (2000). Effects of species-specific cues and the CS-US interval on the topography of the sexually conditioned response. Learning and Motivation, 31, 211235. https://doi.org/10.1006/lmot.2000.1050Google Scholar
Andersson, M., & Simmons, L. W. (2006). Sexual selection and mate choice. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 21(6), 296302. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2006.03.015Google Scholar
Ball, G. F., & Balthazart, J. (2010). Japanese quail as a model system for studying the neuroendocrine control of reproductive and social behaviors. ILAR Journal, 5, 310325. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.51.4.310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkhead, T. (1987). Sperm competition in birds. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 2(9), 268272. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(87)90033-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blass, E.M., & Ganchrow, J. R., &, Steiner, J. E. (1984). Classical conditioning in newborn humans 2–48 hours of age. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 223235. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80060-0Google Scholar
Bonde, J. P., Ernst, E., Jensen, T. K., Hjollund, N. H., Kolstad, H., Henriksen, T. B., … Skakkebaek, N. E. (1998). Relation between semen quality and fertility: A population-based study of 430 first-pregnancy planners. Lancet (London, England), 352(9135), 11721177. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(97)10514-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coria-Avila, G. A., Jones, S. L., Solomon, C. E., Gavrila, A. M., Jordan, G. J., & Pfaus, J. G. (2006). Conditioned partner preference in female rats for strain of male. Physiology and Behavior, 88, 529537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coria-Avila, G. A., Ouimet, A. J., Pacheco, P., Manzo, J., & Pfaus, J. G. (2005). Olfactory conditioned partner preference in the female rat. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 716725. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.716CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornil, C. A., Holloway, K. S., Taziaux, M., & Balthazart, J. (2004). The effects of aromatase inhibition on testosterone-dependent conditioned rhythmic cloacal sphincter movements in male Japanese quail. Physiology and Behavior, 83, 99105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2004.07.011Google Scholar
Cornil, C. A., Taziaux, M., Baillien, M., Ball, G. F., & Balthazart, J. (2006). Rapid effects of aromatase inhibition on male reproductive behaviors in Japanese quail. Hormones and Behavior, 49, 4567. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.05.003Google Scholar
Cusato, B., & Domjan, M. (1998). Special efficacy of sexual conditioned stimuli that include species-typical cues: Tests with a conditioned stimulus preexposure design. Learning and Motivation, 29, 152167.Google Scholar
Domjan, M. (2005). Pavlovian conditioning: A functional perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 179206. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domjan, M., Blesbois, E., & Williams, J. (1998). The adaptive significance of sexual conditioning: Pavlovian control of sperm release. Psychological Science, 9, 411415.Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Cusato, B., & Krause, M. (2004). Learning with arbitrary versus ecological conditioned stimuli: Evidence from sexual conditioning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 232246. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domjan, M., & Galef, B. G. Jr. (1983). Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: Retrospect and prospect. Animal Learning & Behavior, 11, 151161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Domjan, M., & Gutiérrez, G. (2019). The behavior system for sexual learning. Behavioural Processes, 162, 184196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2019.01.013Google Scholar
Domjan, M., & Krause, M. A. (2017). Adaptive specializations and the generality of the laws of classical and instrumental conditioning. In Menzel, R. (Ed.), Learning theory and behavior: Vol. 1, Learning and memory: A comprehensive reference (2nd ed., Byrne, J. H. (Ed.), pp. 180201). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Lyons, R., North, N. C., & Bruell, J. (1986). Sexual Pavlovian conditioned approach behavior in male Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100, 413421. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0735-7036.100.4.413CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Domjan, M., Mahometa, M. J., & Matthews, R. N. (2012). Learning in intimate connections: Conditioned fertility and its role in sexual competition. Socioaffective Neuroscience & Psychology, 2, 110. https://doi.org/10.3402/snp.v2i0.17333Google Scholar
Domjan, M., Mahometa, M. J., & Mills, A. D. (2003). Relative contributions of the male and the female to sexual behavior and reproductive success in the Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 391399. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.117.4.391Google Scholar
Dukas, R. (2013). Effects of learning on evolution: Robustness, innovation and speciation. Animal Behaviour, 85(5), 10231030. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.030Google Scholar
Dunlap, A. S., Austin, M. W., & Figueiredo, A. (2019). Components of change and the evolution of learning in theory and experiment. Animal Behaviour, 147, 157166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2018.05.024Google Scholar
Dunlap, A. S., & Stephens, D. W. (2014). Experimental evolution of prepared learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 1175011755. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1404176111Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J., Fray, P., Kostarczyk, E., Taylor, S., & Stacey, P. (1987). Studies of instrumental behavior with sexual reinforcement in male rats (Rattus norvegicus): I. Control by brief visual stimuli paired with a receptive female. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 101, 395406. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.101.4.395Google Scholar
Gaalema, D. E. (2013). Sexual conditioning in the dyeing poison dart frog (Dendrobates tinctorius ). International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 26, 518. http://hdl.handle.net/1853/33895CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gámez, A. M., & León, S. P. (2018). The role of learning in the oviposition behavior of the silkworm moth (Bombyx mori). Behavioural Processes, 157, 286290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.10.023Google Scholar
Ginsburg, S., & Jablonka, E. (2010). The evolution of associative learning: A factor in the Cambrian explosion. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 266, 1120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2010.06.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J. M., & Desjardins, C. (1980). Classical conditioning: Induction of luteinizing hormone and testosterone secretion in anticipation of sexual activity. Science, 210(4473), 10391041. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7434016Google Scholar
Gutiérrez, G., & Domjan, M. (1997). Differences in the sexual conditioned behavior of male and female Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 135142. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.111.2.135Google Scholar
Gutiérrez, G., & Domjan, M. (2011). Conditioning of sexual proceptivity in female quail: Measures of conditioned place preference. Behavioural Processes, 87, 268273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollis, K. L., Pharr, V. L., Dumas, M. J., Britton, G. B., & Field, J. (1997). Classical conditioning provides paternity advantage for territorial male blue gouramis (Trichogaster trichopterus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 219225. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0735-7036.111.3.219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holloway, K. S., Balthazart, J., & Cornil, C. A. (2005). Androgen mediation of conditioned rhythmic cloacal sphincter movements in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 4957. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.1.49CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerat, E., Janett, F., Sieme, H., Wedekind, C., & Burger, D. (2017). Quality of seminal fluids varies with type of stimulus at ejaculation. Scientific Reports, 7, 18. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep44339Google Scholar
Kelly, C. D., & Jennions, M. D. (2011). Sexual selection and sperm quantity: Meta-analyses of strategic ejaculation. Biological Reviews, 86(4), 863884. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2011.00175.xGoogle Scholar
Kippin, T. E., & Pfaus, J. G. (2001). The nature of the conditioned response mediating olfactory conditioned ejaculatory preference in the male rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 122(1), 1124. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00162-0Google Scholar
Krause, M. A. (2015). Evolutionary perspectives on learning: Conceptual and methodological issues in the study of adaptive specializations. Animal Cognition, 18(4), 807820. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0854-4Google Scholar
Langen, E. M. A., Goerlich-Jansson, V. C., & von Engelhardt, N. (2019). Effects of the maternal and current social environment on female body mass and reproductive traits in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica). Journal of Experimental Biology, 222(5), jeb187005. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.187005Google Scholar
Ledecka, D., Zeman, M., & Okuliarova, M. (2019). Genetic variation in maternal yolk testosterone allocation predicts female mating decisions in Japanese quail. Animal Behaviour, 157, 3542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.08.022Google Scholar
Mahometa, M. J., & Domjan, M. (2005). Classical conditioning increases reproductive success in Japanese quail, Coturnix japonica. Animal Behaviour, 69(4), 983989. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthews, R. N., Domjan, M., Ramsey, M., & Crews, D. (2007). Learning effects on sperm competition and reproductive fitness. Psychological Science, 18(9), 758762. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01974.xGoogle Scholar
McNeilly, A. S., Robinson, I. C., Houston, M. J., & Howie, H. P. (1983). Release of oxytocin and prolactin in response to suckling. British Medical Journal, 286, 257259. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.286.6361.257CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moatt, J. P., Dytham, C., & Thom, M. D. F. (2014). Sperm production responds to perceived sperm competition risk in male Drosophila melanogaster. Physiology and Behavior, 131, 111114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.027Google Scholar
Pandeirada, J. N. S., Fernandes, N. L., Vasconcelos, M., & Nairne, J. S. (2017). Adaptive memory: Remembering potential mates. Evolutionary Psychology, 15, 111. https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704917742807Google Scholar
Paredes, R. G., & Alonso, A. (1997). Sexual behavior regulated (paced) by the female induces conditioned place preference. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111(1), 123128. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.123Google Scholar
Pfaus, J. G., Kippin, T. E., & Centeno, S. (2001). Conditioning and sexual behavior: A review. Hormones and Behavior, 40(2), 291321. https://doi.org/10.1006/hbeh.2001.1686Google Scholar
Pontes, A., Mobley, R. B., Ofria, C., Adami, C., & Dyer, F. C. (2019). The evolutionary origin of associative learning. The American Naturalist, 195(1), E1E19. https://doi.org/10.1086/706252CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutkowska, J., & Adkins-Regan, E. (2009). Learning enhances female control over reproductive investment in the Japanese quail. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1671), 33273334. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.0762Google Scholar
Taziaux, M., Kahn, A., Moore, J., Balthazart, J., & Holloway, K. S. (2008). Enhanced neural activation in brain regions mediating sexual responses following exposure to a conditioned stimulus that predicts copulation. Neuroscience, 151(3), 644658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.10.056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taziaux, M., Lopez, J., Cornil, C. A., Balthazart, J., & Holloway, K. S. (2007). Differential c-fos expression in the brain of male Japanese quail following exposure to stimuli that predict or do not predict the arrival of a female. European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 28352846. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05542.xGoogle Scholar
Timberlake, W. (2001). Motivational modes in behavior systems. In Mowrer, R. R. & Klein, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of contemporary learning theories (pp. 155209). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L., & Willard, D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179(4068), 9092. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.179.4068.90Google Scholar
Zamble, E., Mitchell, J. B., & Findlay, H. (1986). Pavlovian conditioning of sexual arousal: Parametric and background manipulations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 403411. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.12.4.403Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×