Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-mggfc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T09:13:29.097Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Acquisition of Negation in Jamaican

from Part II - The Acquisition of Syntax

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2025

Dany Adone
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Astrid Gabel
Affiliation:
Universität zu Köln
Get access

Summary

De Lisser and Durleman’s first chapter focuses on Bickerton’s hypotheses about language acquisition. Whether articulated in terms of the Language Bioprogram or in terms of default parameter settings of Universal Grammar (1981, 1984, 1999, 2014, 2016), the hypotheses predict that prototypical Creole features will emerge in early stages of child productions. This view thus leads them to expect target-inconsistent utterances during the acquisition of non-Creole languages where such features are not present, and target-consistent utterances in the acquisition of Creole languages. The investigation tests the second of these predictions for negation via an eighteen-month longitudinal study of the spontaneous production of six Jamaican-speaking children between the ages of 18 and 23 months at the start of the research. The findings reveal an absence of target-inconsistent options for the expression of negation, suggesting that children acquiring Jamaican are knowledgeable of the rules governing negation from their earliest negative utterances, be they sentential, constituent or anaphoric. Taken together, these findings suggest that the acquisition of negation in Jamaican follows Bickerton’s predictions, which are also in line with the more general claim that Negative Concord (NC) is a default choice explored in early stages of child grammar regardless of the target (Moscatti 2020; Thornton 2020).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolution, Acquisition and Development of Syntax
Insights from Creole Languages and Beyond
, pp. 119 - 136
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adone, D. (1994). The Acquisition of Mauritian Creole. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, B. (1966). Jamaican Creole Syntax: A Transformational Approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Baptista, M. (2017). Pidgins and Creoles: Syntax. Oxford Handbook Online. Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935345.013.13Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of Language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1984). The language bioprogram hypothesis. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 7: 173188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1999a). How to acquire language without positive evidence: What Acquisitionists can learn from Creoles. In DeGraff, M., (ed.), Language Creation and Language Change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 4974.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (1999b). Creole languages: The Language Bioprogram Hypothesis, and language acquisition. In Ritchie, W. C. & Bhatia, T. K. (eds.), Handbook of Child Language Acquisition. New York: Academic Press, pp. 195220.Google Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2014). More than Nature Needs: Language, Mind, and Evolution. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, D. (2016). Roots of Language (Classics in Linguistics 3). Berlin: Language Science Press.Google Scholar
Bellugi, U. (1967). The acquisition of the system of negation in children’s speech. Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University.Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures in Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. (1988). Parameterized grammatical theory and language acquisition: A study of the acquisition of verb placement and inflection by children and adults. In Flynn, S. & O’Neil, W. (eds.), Linguistic Theory in Second Language Acquisition. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 4775.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Lisser, T. N. (2015). The acquisition of Jamaican Creole: The emergence and transformation of early syntactic systems. Doctoral thesis, University of Geneva, no. L. 835. https://doi.org/10.13097/archive-ouverte/unige:74228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Lisser, T., Durrleman, S., Rizzi, L., & Shlonsky, U.. (2014). The acquisition of Jamaican Creole: A research project. Rivista di Grammatica Generativa, 36, 83101.Google Scholar
De Lisser, T., Durrleman, S., Rizzi, L., & Shlonsky, U. (2016). The acquisition of Jamaican Creole: Null subject phenomenon. Language Acquisition, 23, 261292. https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2015.1115049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deprez, V., & Pierce, A.. (1993). Negation and functional projections in early grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 24(1), 2567.Google Scholar
Durrleman, S. (2008). The Syntax of Jamaican Creole: A Cartographic Perspective. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Hyams, N., & Curtiss, S.. (2002). “No I Understand Negation”: A Preferential Looking Paradigm Study of Early Knowledge of Sentential and Anaphoric Negation. https://linguistics.ucla.edu/people/curtiss/2002%20-%20No%20I%20understand%20negation.pdfGoogle Scholar
Gilkerson, J., Hyams, N., & Curtiss, S.. (2004). On the scope of negation: More evidence for early parameter setting. In van Kampen, Jacqueline & Baauw, Sergio (eds.), Proceedings of GALA 2003. Utrecht: LOT, pp. 175185.Google Scholar
Guasti, M. T., & Rizzi, L.. (2002). Agreement and Tense as distinct syntactic positions: Evidence from acquisition. In Cinque, G. (ed.), The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, Vol. I: Functional Structure in DP and IP. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 167194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, T., & Wexler, K.. (1996). The optional infinitive stage in child English: Evidence from negation. In Clahsen, H. (ed.), Generative Perspectives on Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 142.Google Scholar
Hyams, N. (1983). The acquisition of parameterized grammars. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, City University of New York.Google Scholar
Hyams, N. (1986). Language Acquisition and the Theory of Parameters. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2011). What is the human language faculty? Two views. Language, 87(3), 586624. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2011.0063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U.. (1966). Syntactic regularities in the speech of children. In Lyons, J. & Wales, R. (eds.), Psycholinguistic Papers. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press, pp. 183208.Google Scholar
Miller, K. (2012). Sociolinguistic variation in Brown’s Sarah corpus. Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD), 36, 339348.Google Scholar
Moscati, V. (2020) Children (and some adults) overgeneralize Negative Concord: The case of fragment answers to negative questions in Italian. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 26(1), Article 20.Google Scholar
Patrick, P. (2007). JC Jamaican Patwa (Creole English). Creolica.Google Scholar
Pollock, J.-Y. (1989). Verb movement, Universal Grammar, and the structure of IP. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(3), 365424.Google Scholar
Schütze, C. T. (2010). Linguistic evidence and grammatical theory. WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(2), 206221. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Syea, A. 1993. Null Subject in Mauritian Creole and the ProDrop Parameter. In Byrne, F. & Holm, J. (eds.), Atlantic Meets Pacific: A Global View of Pidginization and Creolization. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 91102.Google Scholar
Thornton, R. (2020). Negation and first language acquisition. In Déprez, V. & M. T. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Negation. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 599614. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198830528.013.36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thornton, R., Notley, A., Moscati, V., & Crain, S. (2016). Two negations for the price of one. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 1(1), Article 45. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.4Google Scholar
Thornton, R., & Tesan, G.. (2013). Sentential negation in early child English. Journal of Linguistics, 49, 367411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Auwera, J., & De Lisser, T. N.. (2019). Negative Concord in Jamaican. Elsevier. Ampersand, 6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amper.2019.100051CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wode, H. (1977). On the systematicity of L1 transfer in L2 acquisition. Proceedings from 1977 Second Language Research Forum (SLRF). Los Angeles: University of California, Department of Applied Linguistics, pp. 160169.Google Scholar
Zhou, P., Crain, S., & Thornton, R.. (2014). Children’s knowledge of double negative structures in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 23, 333359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×