Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T02:03:10.516Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 March 2011

Jürgen Bohnemeyer
Affiliation:
University at Buffalo, State University of New York
Eric Pederson
Affiliation:
University of Oregon
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahn, W. and Bailenson, J. 1996. ‘Mechanism-based explanations of causal attribution: An explanation of conjunction and discounting effect’, Cognitive Psychology 31: 82–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahn, W. and Kalish, C. W. 2000. ‘The role of mechanism beliefs in causal reasoning’, in Keil, F. C. and Wilson, R. A. (eds.), Explanation and Cognition, pp. 199–225. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ahn, W., Kalish, C. W., Medin, D. L., and Gelman, S. A. 1995. ‘The role of covariation versus mechanism information in causal attribution’, Cognition 54: 299–352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Dixon, R. M. W. (eds.) 2006. Serial Verb Constructions: A Cross-linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., and Young, A. J. 2000. ‘Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture’, Language and Cognitive Processes 15(6): 593–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alsina, A., Bresnan, J., and Sells, P. (eds.) 1997. Complex Predicates. Stanford: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Ameka, F. K. 2005a. ‘Ewe serial verb constructions in their grammatical context’, in Aikhenvald, A. Y. and Dixon, R. M. W. (eds.), Serial Verb Constructions: A Cross-linguistic Typology, pp. 124–43. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ameka, F. K. 2005b. ‘Multiverb constructions on the West African littoral: Micro-variation and areal typology’, in Vulchanova, M. and Åfarli, T. A. (eds.), Grammar and Beyond: Essays in honor of Lars Hellan, pp. 15–42. Oslo: Novus Press.Google Scholar
Ameka, F. K. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) 2007. ‘Special issue on locative predicates’, Linguistics 45(5/6).Google Scholar
Andersen, E. S. 1978. ‘Lexical universals in body-part terminology’, in Greenberg, J. H. (ed.), Universals of Human Language, vol. 3, pp. 335–68. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Aronoff, M., Meir, I., and Sandler, W. 2005. ‘The paradox of sign language morphology’, Language 81(2): 301–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aronoff, M., Meir, I., Padden, C., and Sandler, W. 2003. ‘Classifier constructions and morphology in two sign languages’, in Emmorey, K. (ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages, pp. 53–84. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Aronson, J. L. 1971. ‘On the grammar of “CAUSE”’, Synthese 22: 414–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aske, J. 1989. ‘Path predicates in English and Spanish: A closer look’, Proceedings of the Fifteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 1–14. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Bach, E. 1981. ‘On time, tense, and aspect: An essay in English metaphysics’, in Cole, P. (ed.), Radical Pragmatics, pp. 62–81. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baldwin, D. 2005. ‘Discerning intentions: Characterizing the cognitive system at play’, in Homer, B. D. and Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (eds.), The Development of Social Cognition and Communication, pp. 117–44. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barbey, A. K. and Wolff, P. 2006. ‘Causal reasoning from forces’, Proceedings of the 28th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 24–39. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barbey, A. K. and Wolff, P. 2007. ‘Learning causal structure from reasoning’, Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 713–18. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barbey, A. K. and Wolff, P. ms. ‘Composing causal relations in force dynamics’, manuscript, Emory University.
Barwise, J. and Perry, J. 1983. Situations and Attitudes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bavelas, J. B., Kenwood, C., Johnson, T., and Phillips, B. 2002. ‘An experimental study of when and how speakers use gestures to communicate’, Gesture 2(1): 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. and Shovelton, H. 1999a. ‘Do iconic hand gestures really contribute anything to the semantic information conveyed by speech?Semiotica 123(1/2): 1–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. and Shovelton, H. 1999b. ‘Mapping the range of information contained in the iconic hand gestures that accompany spontaneous speech’, Journal of Language and Social Psychology 18(4): 438–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G. and Shovelton, H. 2002. ‘An experimental investigation of some properties of individual iconic gestures that mediate their communicative power’, British Journal of Psychology 93(2): 179–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beebee, H. 2004. ‘Causing and Nothingness’, in Collins, J., Hall, N., and Paul, L. (eds.), Causation and Counterfactuals, pp. 291–308. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berlin, B. and Kay, P. 1969. Basic Color Terms. [Paperback edition, reprinted 1999] Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. and Slobin, D. I. 1994. ‘Filtering and packaging in narrative’, in Berman, R. and Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study, pp. 515–54. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berman, R. A. and Slobin, D. I. 1994b. Relating Events in Narrative: A Crosslinguistic Developmental Study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Berthele, R. 2004. ‘The typology of motion and posture verbs: A variationist account’, in Kortmann, B. (ed.), Dialectology Meets Typology. Dialect Grammar from a Cross-linguistic Perspective, pp. 93–126. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Berthele, R. 2006. Ort und Weg. Die sprachlich Raumreferenz in Varietäten des Deutschen, Rätoromanischen und Französischen. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Biederman, I. 1972. ‘Perceiving real world scenes’, Science 177: 77–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, I. 1987. ‘Recognition-by-components: A theory of human image understanding’, Psychological Review 94: 115–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biederman, I., Rabinowiz, , Glass, J., , A. L., and Stacy, , Jr, E. W.. 1974. ‘On the information extracted from a glance at a scene’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 103: 597–600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K. and Levelt, W. J. M. 1994. ‘Language production. Grammatical encoding’, in Gernsbacher, M. A. (ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics, pp. 405–52. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bock, K., Irwin, D. E., Davidson, D. J., and Levelt, W. J. M. 2003. ‘Minding the clock’, Journal of Memory and Language 48: 653–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. 1999. ‘A questionnaire on event integration’, in Wilkins, D. P. (ed.), “Manual” for the 1999 Field Season, pp. 87–95. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. 2003. ‘The unique vector constraint’, in Zee, E. and Slack, J. (eds.), Representing Direction in Language and Space, pp. 86–110. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. 2004. ‘Split intransitivity, linking, and lexical representation: the case of Yukatek Maya’, Linguistics 42(1): 67–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. and Caelen, M. 1999. ‘The ECOM clips: A stimulus for the linguistic coding of event complexity’, in Wilkins, D. P. (ed.), ‘Manual’ for the 1999 Field Season, pp. 74–86. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J. and Majid, A. 2002. ‘ECOM causality revisited version 4’, in Kita, S. (ed.), 2002 Supplement (Version 3) for the ‘Manual’ for the Field Season 2001, pp. 35–58. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Bohnemeyer, J., Eisenbeiß, S., and Narasimhan, B. 2006. ‘Ways to go: Methodological considerations in Whorfian studies on motion events’, Essex Research Reports in Linguistics 50: 1–20.
Bohnemeyer, J., Enfield, N., Essegbey, J., Ibarretxe, I., Kita, S., Lüpke, F., and Ameka, F. K. 2007. ‘Principles of event representation in language: The case of motion events’, Language 83(3): 495–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L. 2001. ‘Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time', Cognitive Psychology 43(1): 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L. 2003. ‘Linguistic relativity’, in Nadel, L. (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, pp. 917–21. London: Macmillan Press.Google Scholar
Bower, G. H., Black, J. B., and Turner, T. J. 1979. ‘Scripts in memory for text’, Cognitive Psychology 11: 177–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1978. ‘Systematizing semantic knowledge: Changes over time in the child's organization of word meaning’, Child Development 49(4): 987–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. 1982. ‘Starting to talk worse: Clues to language acquisition from children's late speech errors’, in Strauss, S. and Stavy, R. (eds.), U-shaped Behavioral Growth, pp. 101–46. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. 2005. ‘Why can't you “open” a nut or “break” a cooked noodle? Learning covert object categories in action word meanings’, in Gershkoff-Stowe, L. and Rakison, D. (eds.), Building Object Categories in Developmental Time, pp. 209–44. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bowerman, M. and Choi, S. 2001. ‘Shaping meanings for language: universal and language-specific in the acquisition of spatial semantic categories’, in Bowerman, M. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, pp. 475–511. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, M. and Pederson, E. ms. ‘Cross-linguistic perspectives on topological spatial relations’, Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.
Bowerman, M., Brown, P., Eisenbeiß, S., Narasimhan, B., and Slobin, D. I. 2002. ‘Putting things in places. Developmental consequences of linguistic typology’, in Clark, E. V. (ed.), Space In Language. Location, Motion, Path, and Manner. The Proceedings of the 31st Stanford Child Language Research Forum, pp. S1–S122. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Braun, J. 2003. ‘Natural scenes upset the visual applecart’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 7–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brennan, M. 1992. ‘The visual world of BSL: An introduction’, in Brien, D. (ed.), Dictionary of British Sign Language/English, pp. 1–133. London: Faber and Faber.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J. 1982. ‘Polyadicity’, in Bresnan, J. (ed.), The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations, pp. 149–72. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, C. H. 1976. ‘General principles of human anatomical partonomy and speculations on the growth of partonomic nomenclature’, American Ethnologist 3: 400–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. 2006. ‘A sketch of the grammar of space in Tzeltal’, in Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. (eds.), Grammars of Space: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity, pp. 230–72. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, P. 2008. ‘Verb specificity and argument realization in Tzeltal child language’, in Bowerman, M. and Brown, P. (eds.), Crosslinguistic Perspectives on Argument Structure: Implications for Language Acquisition, pp. 167–89. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brown, P. forthcoming. ‘To “put” or to “take”? Verb semantics in Tzeltal placement and removal expressions’, in A. Kopecka and B. Narasimhan (eds.), Events of “putting” and “taking”: A Crosslinguistic Perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Brown, R. W. and Lenneberg, E. H. 1958. ‘Studies in linguistic relativity’, in Maccoby, E., Newcomb, T. H., and Hartley, E. L. (eds.), Readings in Social Psychology 3rd edn, pp. 9–18. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Bruce, L. 1986. The Alamblak Language of Papua New Guinea (East Sepik). Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Bruce, L. 1988. ‘Serialization: from syntax to lexicon’, Studies in Language 12(1):19–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruce, V., Green, P. R., and Georgson, M. A. 1996. Visual Perception: Physiology, Psychology, and Ecology, 3rd edn. Hove, UK: Psychology Press, Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brugman, H. and Kita, S. 1995. ‘Impact of digital video technology on transcription: a case of spontaneous gesture transcription’, KODIKAS/CODE: Ars Semiotica. An International Journal of Semiotics 18: 95–112.Google Scholar
Bullock, M., Gelman, R., and Baillargeon, R. 1982. ‘The development of causal reasoning’, in Friedman, W. (ed.), The Developmental Psychology of Time, pp. 209–55. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Carlson, G. 1984. ‘Thematic roles and their role in semantic interpretation’, Linguistics 22: 259–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, M. and Stutterheim, C. 2003. ‘Typology and information organization: perspective taking and language-specific effects in the construal of events’, in Ramat, A. G. (ed.), Typology and Second Language Acquisition, pp. 365–402. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Carroll, M., Murcia-Serra, J., Watorek, M., and Bendiscoli, A. 2000. ‘The relevance of information organization to second language acquisition studies: The descriptive discourse of advanced adult learners of German’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22(3): 441–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, M., Rossdeutscher, A., Lambert, M., and Stutterheim, C. 2008. ‘Subordination in narratives and macrostructural planning: A comparative point of view’, in Fabricius Hansen, C. and Ramm, W. (eds.), Subordination versus Coordination in Sentence and Text – From a Crosslinguistic Perspective, pp. 161–84. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, M., Rossdeutscher, A., Lambert, M., and Stutterheim, C. 1993. ‘The representation of spatial configurations in English and German and the grammatical structure of locative and anaphoric expressions’, Linguistics 31: 1011–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Casati, R. and Varzi, A. C. 1999. Parts and Places: The Structures of Spatial Representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Casati, R. and Varzi, A. C. (eds.) 1996. Events. Brookfield, VT: Dartmouth.Google Scholar
Cassell, J., McNeill, D., and McCullough, K.-E. 1999. ‘Speech–gesture mismatches: Evidence for one underlying representation of linguistic and nonlinguistic information’, Pragmatics and Cognition 7(1): 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. 1979. ‘The flow of thought and the flow of language’, in Givón, T. (ed.), Syntax and Semantics, vol. 12: Discourse and Syntax, pp. 159–81. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. 1980. ‘The deployment of consciousness in the production of a narrative’, in Chafe, W. (ed.), The Pear Stories: Cognitive, Cultural and Linguistic Aspects of Narrative Production. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. 1987. ‘Cognitive constraints on information flow’, in Tomlin, R. (ed.), Coherence and Grounding in Discourse, pp. 21–51. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, W. 1994. Discourse, Consciousness and Time. The Flow and Displacement of Conscious Experience in Speaking and Writing. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Chaigneau, S. E. and Barbey, A. K. 2008. ‘Assessing psychological theories of causal meaning and inference’, in Love, B. C., McRae, K., and Sloutsky, V. M. (eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 1111–16. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, S. H., Freyd, J. J., and Shiffrar, M. 1996. ‘Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 22: 916–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Cheng, P. W. 1997. ‘From covariation to causation: A causal power theory’, Psychological Review 104: 367–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheng, P. W. and Novick, L. R. 1991. ‘Causes versus enabling conditions’, Cognition 40: 83–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, P. W. and Novick, L. R. 1992. ‘Covariation in natural causal induction’, Psychological Review 99: 365–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chenu, F. and Jisa, H. 2006. ‘Caused motion constructions and semantic generality in early acquisition of French’, in Clark, E. V. and Kelly, B. F. (eds.), Constructions in Acquisition, vol. 174, pp. 233–61. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Chierchia, G. 1998a. ‘Reference to kinds across languages’, Natural Language Semantics 6: 339–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G. 1998b. ‘Plurality of mass nouns and the notion of semantic parameter’, in Rothstein, S. (ed.), Events and Grammar, pp. 53–103. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, S., McDonough, M., Bowerman, M., and Mandler, J. 1999. ‘Early sensitivity to language-specific spatial categories in English and Korean’, Cognitive Development 14(2): 241–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Chung, S. and Ladusaw, W. A. 2004. Restriction and saturation. Linguistic Inquiry Monographs 42. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clahsen, H. 1982. Spracherwerb in der Kindheit: Eine Untersuchung zur Entwicklung der Syntax bei Kleinkindern. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Gerrig, R. J. 1990. ‘Quotations as demonstrations’, Language 66: 764–805.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Wilkes-Gibbs, D. 1986. ‘Referring as a collaborative process’, Cognition 22(1): 1–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H., H., Carpenter, P. A., and Just, M. A. 1973. ‘On the meeting of semantics and perception’, in Chase, W. G. (ed.), Visual Information Processing, pp. 311–82. New York: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, C. E. and Ebbesen, E. B. 1979. ‘Observational goals and schema activation: a theoretical framework for behavior perception’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 15: 305–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, B. 1976. Aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Comrie, B. 1981. Language Universals and Linguistic Typology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Conditt, M. A., Gandolfo, F., and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. 1997. ‘The motor system does not learn the dynamics of the arm by rote memorization of past experience’, Journal of Neurophysiology 78: 554–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, W. 1990. ‘Possible verbs and the structure of events’, in Tsohatzidis, S. L. (ed.), Meanings and Prototypes: Studies in Linguistic Categorization, pp. 48–73. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Dahl, Ö. 1985. Tense and Aspect Systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Davenport, J. L. and Potter, M. C. 2004. ‘Scene consistency in object and background perception’, Psychological Science 15: 559–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, C. 2003. Les “verbs of putting”: Typologie, schéma syntaxique et organisation sémantique des constructions prépositionnelles en anglais contemporain. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Université de Poitiers, Poitiers.
Davidson, D. 1967. ‘The logical form of action sentences’, in Rescher, N. (ed.), The Logic of Decision and Action, pp. 81–95. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, P. R. and Wolpert, D. M. 2004. ‘Internal models underlying grasp can be additively combined’, Experimental Brain Research 155: 334–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, J. 1981. Kobon. Lingua Descriptiva Series 3. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Ruiter, J. P. 2000. ‘The production of gesture and speech’, in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture: Window into Thought and Action, pp. 284–311. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruiter, J. P. 2007. ‘Postcards from the mind: The relationship between speech, gesture and thought’, Gesture 7(1): 21–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delorme, A., Richard, G., and Fabre-Thorpe, M. 2000. ‘Ultra-rapid categorization of natural scenes does not rely on color cues: a study in monkeys and humans’, Vision Research 40: 2187–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deringil, S. 2002. İktidarın Sembolleri ve İdeoloji: II. Abdülhamid Dönemi (1876–1909), [The symbols of power and ideology: The era of II. Abdülhamid (1876–1909)] İstanbul: Yapi Kredi Press.Google Scholar
Deubel, H. and Schneider, W. X. 1996. ‘Saccade target selection and object recognition: Evidence for a common attentional mechanism’, Vision Research 36: 1827–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, C. 2002. Complex predicates in Tsafiki. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Oregon.
Dixon, R. M. W. 1994. Ergativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobel, C. ms. ‘Interaction of vision and language in the description of action events’. Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster.
Dobel, C., Gumnior, H., Bölte, J., and Zwitserlood, P. 2007. ‘Describing scenes hardly seen’, Acta Psychologica 125: 129–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowe, P. 2000. Physical Causation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowe, P. 2001. ‘A counterfactual theory of prevention and “causation” by omission’, Australasian Journal of Philosophy 79: 216–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowe, P. 2007. ‘Causal processes’, in Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu.Google Scholar
Dowty, D. 1979. Word Meaning and Montague Grammar. Synthese Language Library. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowty, D. 1991. ‘Thematic roles and argument selection’, Language 67: 547–619.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Du Bois, J. 1987. ‘The discourse basis of ergativity’, Language 63(4): 805–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, S. 1996. Grammatical form and ‘thinking-for-speaking’ in Mandarin Chinese and English: An analysis based on speech-accompanying gesture. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Chicago, Chicago.
Durie, M. 1997. ‘Grammatical structure in verb serialization’, in Alsina, A., Bresnan, J. and Sells, P. (eds.), Complex Predicates, pp. 289–354. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. 2002. Language, Cognition, and the Brain: Insights from Sign Language Research. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. (ed.) 2003. Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K. and Falgier, B. 1999. ‘Talking about space with space: Describing environments in ASL’, in Winston, E. (ed.), Storytelling and Conversation: Discourse in Deaf Communities, pp. 3–26. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Engberg-Pedersen, E. 1993. Space in Danish Sign Language: The Semantics and Morphosyntax of the Use of Space in a Visual Language. Hamburg: Signum Press.Google Scholar
Engberg-Pedersen, E. 1995. ‘Point of view expressed through shifters’, in Emmorey, K. and Reilly, J. (eds.), Language, Gesture and Space, pp. 133–55. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fabre-Thorpe, M., Delorme, A., Marlot, C., and Thorpe, S. 2001. ‘A limit to the speed of processing in ultra-rapid visual categorization of novel natural scenes’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13: 171–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fair, D. 1979. ‘Causation and the flow of energy’, Erkenntnis 14: 219–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farr, C. 1999. The Interface Between Syntax and Discourse in Korafe, a Papuan Language of Papua New Guinea. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F. 2000. ‘Syntax in language production: An approach using tree-adjoining grammars’, in Wheeldon, L. (ed.), Aspects of Language Production, pp. 291–330. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fieandt, K. and Gibson, J. J. 1959. ‘The sensitivity of the eye to two kinds of continuous transformation of a shadow pattern’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 57: 344–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. 1968. ‘The case for case’, in Bach, E. and Harms, R. T. (eds.), Universals of Linguistic Theory, pp. 1–90. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Google Scholar
Fillmore, C. J. 1972. ‘Subjects, speakers and roles’, in Davidson, D. and Harman, G. (eds.), Semantics of Natural Language, pp. 1–24. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Finkbeiner, M., Nicol, J., Greth, D., and Nakamura, K. 2002. ‘The role of language in memory for actions’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 31(5): 447–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fodor, J. 1970. ‘Three reasons not to derive “kill” from “cause to die”’, Linguistic Inquiry 1: 429–438.Google Scholar
Foley, W. and Olson, M. 1985. ‘Clausehood and verb serialization’, in Nichols, J. and Woodbury, A. C. (eds.), Grammar Inside and Outside the Clause, pp. 17–60. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Foley, W. and Valin Jr., R. D. 1984. Functional Syntax and Universal Grammar. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Foot, P. 1967. ‘The problem of abortion and the doctrine of double effect’, Oxford Review 5: 5–15. Reprinted in Steinbock, B. and Norcross, A. (eds.), Killing and Letting Die, 2nd edn, pp. 266–79. New York: Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
Frey, W. 2000. ‘Über die syntaktische Position des Satztopiks im Deutschen’, Issues on Topics. ZAS Papers in Linguistics 20: 137–72.Google Scholar
Freyd, J. J., Pantzer, T. M., and Cheng, J. L. 1988. ‘Representing statics as forces in equilibrium’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117: 395–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridman-Mintz, B. and Liddell, S. K. 1998. ‘Sequencing mental spaces in an ASL narrative’, in Koenig, J. P. (ed.), Discourse and Cognition: Bridging the Gap, pp. 255–68. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gennari, S. P., Sloman, S. A., Malt, B. C., and Fitch, W. 2002. ‘Motion events in language and cognition’, Cognition 83(1): 49–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gernsbacher, M. A. 1997. ‘Coherence cues mapping during comprehension’, in Costermans, J. and Fayol, M. (eds.), Processing Interclausal Relationships: Studies in the Production and Comprehension of Text, pp. 3–22. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Givón, T. 1979. ‘From discourse to syntax: grammar as a processing strategy’, in Givón, T. (ed.), Syntax and Semantics, vol. 12: Discourse and Syntax, pp. 81–112. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Givón, T. 1984. Syntax. A Functional-typological Introduction, vol. 1. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 1990. ‘Verb serialization in Tok Pisin and Kalam: a comparative study of temporal packaging’, in Verhaar, J. (ed.), Melanesian Pidgin and Tok Pisin, pp. 19–56. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 1991a. ‘Serial verbs and event cognition in Kalam: an empirical study of cultural relativity’, in Lefebvre, C. (ed.), Serial Verbs: Grammatical, Comparative and Universal Grammar, pp. 137–84. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, T. 1991b. ‘Serial verbs and the mental reality of “event”’, in Traugott, E. C. and Heine, B. (eds.), Approaches to Grammaticalization, vol. 1, pp. 81–227. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glanemann, R., 2008. ‘To See or Not to See: Action Scenes Out of the Corner of the Eye’. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Münster, http://miami.uni-meunster.de/resolver/urn:nbn:de:hbz:6-84599565282.
Gleitman, L. R. 1990. ‘The structural sources of verb meanings’, Language Acquisition, 1(1): 3–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godfrey-Smith, P. forthcoming. ‘Causal pluralism’, in Beebee, H., Hitchcock, C., and Menzies, P. (eds.), Oxford Handbook of Causation.
Goldberg, A. 1995. Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S. 2003. Hearing Gesture: How Our Hands Help Us Think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Goldman-Eisler, F. 1968. Psycholinguistics: Experiments in Spontaneous Speech. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goldstein, E. B. 2002. Sensation and Perception. Wadsworth: Thomson Learning.Google Scholar
Goldvarg, E. and Johnson-Laird, P. 2001. ‘Naive causality: A mental model theory of causal meaning and reasoning’, Cognitive Science 25: 565–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gopnik, A., Glymour, C., Sobel, D., Shulz, L., Kushnir, T., and Danks, D. 2004. ‘A theory of causal learning in children: Causal maps and Bayes nets’, Psychological Review 111: 1–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grace, G. 1981. An Essay on Language. Columbia, SC: Hornbeam Press.Google Scholar
Grace, G. 1987. The Linguistic Construction of Reality. London/New York/Sydney: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
Graham, J. A. and Argyle, M. 1975. ‘A cross-cultural study of the communication of extra-verbal meaning by gestures’, International Journal of Psychology 10(1): 56–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. 1998. ‘Bilingualism and thought’, Psychologica Belgica 38(3/4): 251–76.Google Scholar
Griffin, Z. M. 2001. ‘Gaze durations during speech reflect word selection and phonological encoding’, Cognition 82: B1–B14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffin, Z. M. 2004. ‘Why look? Reasons for eye movements related to language production’, in Henderson, J. M. and Ferreira, F. (eds.), The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual World, pp. 213–48. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Griffin, Z. M. and Bock, K. 2000. ‘What the eyes say about speaking’, Psychological Science 4: 274–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guariglia, C. and Antonucci, G. 1992. ‘Personal and extrapersonal space: A case of neglect dissociation’, Neuropsychologia 30(11): 1001–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gullberg, M. 1998. Gesture as a Communication Strategy in Second Language Discourse. A Study of Learners of French and Swedish. Lund: Lund University Press.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. 2003. ‘Gestures, referents, and anaphoric linkage in learner varieties’, in Dimroth, C. and Starren, M. (eds.), Information Structure and the Dynamics of Language Acquisition, pp. 311–28. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. 2006. ‘Handling discourse: Gestures, reference tracking, and communication strategies in early L2’, Language Learning 56(1): 155–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gullberg, M. 2009. ‘Why gestures are relevant to the multilingual mental lexicon’, in Pavlenko, A. (ed.), The Multilingual Mental Lexicon, pp. 161–84. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. ms. ‘What learners mean. Gestures and semantic reorganisation of placement verbs in advanced second language production’.
Gullberg, M. and Burenhult, N. forthcoming. ‘Probing the linguistic encoding of placement and removal events in Swedish’, in Kopecka, A. and Narasimhan, B. (eds.), Events of “putting” and “taking”: A Crosslinguistic Perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Gullberg, M. and Narasimhan, B. 2010. ‘What gestures reveal about the development of Dutch children's placement verbs’, Cognitive Linguistics 21: 239–62.
Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.) 1996a. Rethinking Linguistic Relativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. 1996b. ‘Introduction: Linguistic relativity re-examined’, in Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, pp. 1–18. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, N. 2004. ‘Two concepts of causation’, in Collins, J., Hall, N., and Paul, L. (eds.), Causation and Counterfactuals, pp. 225–76. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hansson, K. and Bruce, B. 2002. ‘Verbs of placement in Swedish children with SLI’, International Journal of Communication Disorders 37(4): 401–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hard, B. M., Recchia, G., and Tversky, B. ms. ‘The shape of action’.
Hard, B. M., Tversky, B., and Lang, D. S. 2006. ‘Making sense of abstract events: Building event schemas’, Memory and Cognition 34: 1221–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardin, C. L. and Maffi, L. (eds.) 1997. Color Categories in Thought and Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hasegawa, Y. 1996. ‘A study of Japanese clause linkage: the connective TE in Japanese’, Studies in Japanese Linguistics 5. Stanford: CSLI.Google Scholar
Hayashi, A. 2003. Lexicalization of motion events in Japanese. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Oregon.
Hazout, I. 2004. ‘The syntax of existential constructions’, Linguistic Inquiry 35: 393–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heeschen, V. 2001. ‘Event-formulas: sentences as minimal narratives’, in Pawley, A., Ross, M., and Tryon, D. (eds.), The Boy from Bundaberg: Studies in Melanesian Linguistics in Honour of Tom Dutton, pp. 155–73. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Hegarty, M. 2004. ‘Mechanical reasoning by mental simulation’, TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences 8: 280–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heider, F. and Simmel, M. 1944. ‘An experimental study of apparent behavior’, American Journal of Psychology 57: 243–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henderson, J. M. and Ferreira, F. 2004a. ‘Scene perception for psycholinguists’, in Henderson, J. M. and Ferreira, F. (eds.), The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual Word, pp. 1–58. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Henderson, J. M. and Ferreira, F. (eds.) 2004b. The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action: Eye Movements and the Visual Word. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Hetzron, H. 1975. ‘The presentative movement, or why the ideal word order is V.S.O.P.’, in Li, C. N. (ed.), Word Order and Word Order Change, pp. 345–88. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Heuer, H. and Keele, S. W. (eds.) 1996. Handbook of Perception and Action, vol. 2: Motor Skills. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hickmann, M. 2007. ‘Static and dynamic location in French: Developmental and crosslinguistic perspectives’, in Aurnague, M., Hickmann, M., and Vieu, L. (eds.), The Categorization of Spatial Entities in Language and Cognition, pp. 205–31. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickmann, M. and Hendriks, H. 2006. ‘Static and dynamic location in French and English’, First Language 26(1): 103–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higginbotham, J. 2000. ‘On events in linguistic semantics’, in Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F., and Varzi, A. C. (eds.), Speaking of Events, pp. 49–79. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F., and Varzi, A. C. 2000. Speaking of Events. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hinder, M. R. and Milner, T. E. 2003. ‘The case for an internal dynamics model versus equilibrium point control in human movement’, Journal of Physiology 549: 953–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hitchcock, C. 2001. ‘The intransitivity of causation revealed in equations and graphs’, Journal of Philosophy 98: 273–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitchcock, C. 2002. ‘Probabilistic causation’, Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/causation-probabilistic/
Hoffman, D. D. 2000. Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
Hoffman, D. D. and Richards, W. A. 1984. ‘Parts of recognition’, Cognition 18: 65–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, J. E. and Subramaniam, B. 1995. ‘The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements’, Perception and Psychophysics 57: 787–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holler, J. and Beattie, G. 2003. ‘Pragmatic aspects of representational gestures. Do speakers use them to clarify verbal ambiguity for the listener?’, Gesture 3(2): 127–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollingworth, A. and Henderson, J. 1998. ‘Does consistent scene context facilitate object perception?’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 127: 398–415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hume, D. 1978 (1739). A Treatise of Human Nature, ed. by L. A. Selby-Bigge, 2nd edn, revised by P. H. Nidditch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Imamizu, H., Uno, Y., and Kawato, M. 1995. ‘Internal representations of the motor apparatus: Implications from generalization in visuomotor learning’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 21: 1174–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Irwin, D. E. 1992. ‘Memory for position and identity across eye movements’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18: 307–17.Google Scholar
Irwin, D. E. and Gordon, R. D. 1998. ‘Eye movements, attention and trans-saccadic memory’, Visual Cognition 5: 127–55.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 1983. Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. 1990. Semantic Structures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. J., Wald, J., and Pittenger, J. B. 1986. ‘Apprehending pictorial events’, in McCabe, V. and Balzano, G. J. (eds.), Event Cognition: An Ecological Perspective, pp. 117–33. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jespersen, O. 1965. A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles, vol. VI: Morphology. London: George Allen and Unwin Ltd.Google Scholar
Johansson, G. 1973. ‘Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis’, Perception and Psychophysics 14: 201–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johansson, G. 1975. ‘Visual motion perception’, Scientific American 232(6): 76–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamp, H. 1979. ‘Events, instants and temporal reference’, in Bauerle, R., Egli, U., and Stechow, A. (eds.), Semantics from Different Points of View, pp. 27–54. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
Kamp, H. and Reyle, U. 1993. From Discourse to Logic: Introduction to Model Theoretic Semantics of Natural Language, Formal Logic and Discourse Representation Theory. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Kaufman, L. 1974. Sight and Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kawato, M. 1999. ‘Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning’, Current Opinion in Neurobiology 9: 718–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay, P. and Kempton, W. 1984. ‘What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis?’, American Anthropologist 86(1): 65–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kellerman, E. 1995. ‘Crosslinguistic influence: Transfer to nowhere?’, Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 15: 125–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, S. D., Barr, D. J., Breckinridge Church, R., and Lynch, K. 1999. ‘Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: The role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory’, Journal of Memory and Language 40(4): 577–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemmer, S. and Verhagen, A. 1994. ‘The grammar of causatives and the conceptual structure of events’, Cognitive Linguistics 5: 115–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. 1980. ‘Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance’, in Key, M. R. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Communication, pp. 207–27. The Hague: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. 2004. Gesture. Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. 1999. ‘Japanese enter/exit verbs without motion semantics’, Studies in Language 23(2): 307–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S. and Özyürek, A. 2003. ‘What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking’, Journal of Memory and Language 48(1): 16–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., Gijn, I., and Hulst, H. 1998. ‘Movement phases in signs and co-speech gestures, and their transcription by human coders’, in Wachsmuth, I. and Fröhlich, M. (eds.), Gesture and Sign Language in Human–Computer Interaction, pp. 23–35. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, W. 1994. Time in Language. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Klein, W. 2006. ‘On finiteness’, in Geenhoven, V. (ed.), Semantics in Acquisition, pp. 245–72. Dordrecht: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, W., Li, P., and Hendricks, H. 2000. ‘Aspect and assertion in Mandarin Chinese’, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 18: 723–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopecka, A. and Narasimhan, B. (eds.) forthcoming. Events of “putting” and “taking”: A Crosslinguistic Perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRef
Kosslyn, S. M. 1980. Image and Mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. K., Chen, Y., and Gottesman, R. F. 2000. ‘Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model’, in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 261–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kreysa, H., Glanemann, R., Bölte, J., Zwitserlood, P., and Dobel, C. ms. ‘Where is the action? An eyetracking study on the description of photorealistic events’.
Krifka, M. 1998. ‘The origins of telicity’, in Rothstein, S. (ed.), Events and Grammar, pp. 197–235. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruschke, J. K. and Fragassi, M. M. 1996. ‘The perception of causality: Feature binding in interacting objects’, in Cottrell, G. W. (ed.), Proceedings of the Eighteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 441–6. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Küntay, A. and Slobin, D. I. 1996. ‘Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition’, in Slobin, D. I.Gerhardt, J.Kyratzis, A., and Guo, J. (eds.), Social Interaction, Social Context, and Language: Essays in Honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp, pp. 265–86. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kurby, C. A., Zacks, J. M., Shriver, S., Mehta, R., and Brewer, S. 2008. ‘Event memory and hierarchical segmentation in younger and older adults’, Cognitive Aging Conference, Atlanta,Georgia.Google Scholar
Kurtzer, I., Herter, T. M., and Scott, S. H. 2005. ‘Random change in cortical load representation suggests distinct control of posture and movement’, Nature Neuroscience 8: 498–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heij, W. 2005. ‘Selection processes in monolingual and bilingual lexical access’, in Kroll, J. F. and Groot, A. M. (eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism. Psycholinguistic Approaches, pp. 289–307. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Labov, W. 1973. Language in the Inner City. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Lambrecht, K. 1994. Information Structure and Sentence Form: Topic, Focus, and the Mental Representation of Discourse Referents. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, J. 2007. Kalam Serial Verb Constructions. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Lascarides, A. 1992. ‘Knowledges, causality, and temporal representation’, Linguistics 30(5): 941–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemmens, M. 2002a. ‘The semantic network of Dutch posture verbs’, in Newman, J. (ed.), The Linguistics of Sitting, Standing, and Lying, pp. 103–39. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lemmens, M. 2002b. ‘Tracing referent location in oral picture descriptions’, in Wilson, A., Rayson, P. and McEnery, T. (eds.), A Rainbow of Corpora. Corpus Linguistics and the Languages of the World, pp. 73–85. München: Lincom-Europa.Google Scholar
Lemmens, M. 2006. ‘Caused posture: experiential patterns emerging from corpus research’, in Stefanowitsch, A. and Gries, S. (eds.), Corpora in Cognitive Linguistics. Corpus-based Approaches to Syntax and Lexis, pp. 263–98. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. M. 1984. ‘Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants’, Perception 13: 287–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. M. and Keeble, S. 1987. ‘Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?’, Cognition 25: 265–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lesser, H. 1977. ‘The growth of perceived causality in children’, The Journal of Genetic Psychology 130: 145–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. 1989. Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. 1999. ‘A theory of lexical access in speech production’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 1–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, B. 1993. English Verb Classes and Alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B. 2007. ‘The lexical semantics of verbs I: Introduction’, 2007 Summer Institute of Linguistics at Stanford University, manuscript handout.
Levin, B. and Rappaport Hovav, M. 1994. ‘A preliminary analysis of causative verbs in English’, Lingua 92: 35–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levin, B. and Rappaport Hovav, M. 1995. Unaccusativity: At the Syntax-Lexical Semantics Interface. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levin, B. and Rappaport Hovav, M. 1996. ‘From lexical semantics to argument realization’, in Borer, H. (ed.), Handbook of Morphosyntax and Argument Structure. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 1996. ‘Frames of reference and Molyneux's question: Cross-linguistic evidence’, in Bloom, P., Peterson, M. A., Nadel, L., and Garrett, M. (eds.), Space and Language, pp. 109–69. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 2000. ‘Yélî dnye and the theory of basic color terms’, Journal of Linguistic Anthropology 10: 3–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. 2003. Space in Language and Cognition: Explorations in Cognitive Diversity, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C., Meira, S., and the Language and Cognition Group 2003. ‘Natural concepts’ in the spatial topological domain – adpositional meanings in cross-linguistic perspective: an exercise in semantic typology', Language 79(3): 485–516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. 2006a. ‘The background to the study of the language of space’, in Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. (eds.), Grammars of Space. Explorations in Cognitive Diversity, pp. 1–23. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. (eds.) 2006b. Grammars of Space: Towards a Semantic Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. T. and McNeill, D. 1992. ‘Speech, gesture, and discourse’, Discourse Processes 15(3): 277–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. 1973. ‘Causation’, Journal of Philosophy 70: 556–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. 1986. On The Plurality of Worlds. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. 2000. ‘Causation as influence’, Journal of Philosophy 97: 182–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, F. F., Rullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. 2002. ‘Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 99: 9596–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K. 1994. ‘Tokens and surrogates’, in Ahlgren, I., Bergman, B., and Brennan, M. (eds.), Perspectives on Sign Language Structure. Papers from the 5th International Symposium on Sign Language Research held in Salamanca, Spain, May 25–30, 1992, vol. 1, pp. 105–19. Durham: Isla.Google Scholar
Liddell, S. K. 1995. ‘Real, surrogate, and token space: Grammatical consequences in ASL’, in Emmorey, K. and Reilly, J. (eds.), Language, Gesture, and Space, pp. 19–41. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Liddell, S. K. 2003. Grammar, Gesture, and Meaning in American Sign Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K. and Metzger, M. 1998. ‘Gesture in sign language discourse’, Journal of Pragmatics 30: 657–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lillo-Martin, D. 1995. ‘The point of view predicate in American Sign Language’, in Emmorey, K. and Reilly, J. (eds.), Language, Gesture, and Space, pp. 155–70. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lillo-Martin, D. and Klima, E. 1990. ‘Pointing out differences: ASL pronouns in syntactic theory’, in Fischer, S. and Siple, P. (eds.), Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, vol. 1, pp. 191–210. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Livengood, J. and Machery, E. 2007. ‘The folk probably don't think what you think they think: Experiments on causation by absence’, Midwest Studies in Philosophy 31: 107–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, E. 1980 [1996]. Eyewitness Testimony. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lucy, J. A. 1992. Language Diversity and Thought: A Reformulation of the Linguistic Relativity Hypothesis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucy, J. A. and Gaskins, S. 2001. ‘Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: A comparative approach’, in Levinson, S. C. and Bowerman, M. (eds.), Language Acquisition and Conceptual Development, pp. 257–83. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luhtala, A. 2002. ‘On definitions in ancient grammar’, in Swiggers, P. and Wouters, A. (eds.), Grammatical Theory and Philosophy of Language in Antiquity, pp. 257–85. Leuven: Peeters Publishers.Google Scholar
Mackie, J. L. 1974. The Cement of the Universe. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Majid, A., Staden, M., Boster, J., and Bowerman, M. 2004. ‘Event categorization: A cross-linguistic perspective’, in Forbus, K., Gentner, D., and Regier, T. (eds.), Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 885–90. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Majnep, I. S. and Bulmer, R. 1983. ‘Some food plants in our Kalam forests’, Department of Anthropology Working Papers no. 63, University of Auckland.
Majnep, I. S. and Bulmer, R. 1990. Aps basd skop kmn ak pak ñbelgpal. Kalam hunting traditions, Vols 1–6, ed. by A. Pawley. University of Auckland. Department of Anthropology Working Papers nos. 85–90.Google Scholar
Majnep, I. S. and Bulmer, R. n.d. Kalam hunting traditions. Department of Anthropology Working Papers nos. 7–12, ed. by Pawley, A.. Printout. Department of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.
Majnep, I. S. and Pawley, A. n.d. Kalam plant lore. Computer printout. Department of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.
Mandel, D. R. and Lehman, D. R. 1996. ‘Counterfactual thinking and ascriptions of cause and preventability’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 71: 450–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margetts, A. and Austin, P. 2007. ‘Three-participant events in the languages of the world: towards a cross-linguistic typology’, Linguistics 45: 393–451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matsumoto, Y. 2003. ‘Typologies of lexicalization patterns and event integration: Clarifications and reformulations’, in Chiba, S. (ed.), Empirical and Theoretical Investigations into Language: A Festschrift for Masaru Kajita, pp. 403–18. Tokyo: Kaitakusha.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. and Jaques, J. 2000. ‘Gesture production during stuttered speech: insights into the nature of gesture–speech integration’, in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 199–214. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayberry, R. I. and Nicoladis, E. 2000. ‘Gesture reflects language development: Evidence from bilingual children’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 9(6): 192–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, M. 1969. Frog, where are you?New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
McCabe, V. and Balzano, G. J. (eds.) 1986. Event Cognition: An Ecological Perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McClave, E. 2001. ‘The relationship between spontaneous gestures of the hearing and American Sign Language’. Gesture 1:1, 51–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, B. H. 1982. Aspects of the American Sign Language predicate system. Unpublished PhD dissertation, State University of New York, Buffalo.
McGrath, S. 2003. ‘Causation and the making/allowing distinction’, Philosophical Studies 114: 81–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrath, S. 2005. ‘Causation by omission: A dilemma’, Philosophical Studies 123: 125–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McMahan, J. 1993. ‘Killing, letting die, and withdrawing aid’, Ethics 103: 250–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNally, L. 2009. ‘Properties, entity correlates of properties, and existentials’, in Giannakidou, A. and Rathert, M. (eds.), Quantification, Definiteness, and Nominalization, pp. 163–87. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 1985. ‘So you think gestures are nonverbal?’, Psychological Review 92(3): 271–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. 1992. Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2000a. ‘Analogic/Analytic representations and cross-linguistic differences in thinking for speaking’, Cognitive Linguistics 11(1/2): 43–60.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2000b. ‘Growth points, catchments, and contexts’, Cognitive Studies. Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society 7(1): 22–36.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. 2005. Gesture and Thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. and Duncan, S. D. 2000. ‘Growth points in thinking-for-speaking’, in McNeill, D. (ed.), Language and Gesture, pp. 141–61. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. and Levy, E. T. 1982. ‘Conceptual representations in language activity and gesture’, in Jarvella, R. J. and Klein, W. (eds.), Speech, Place, and Action. Studies in Deixis and Related Topics, pp. 271–95. Chichester: John Wiley.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., Levy, E. T., and Cassell, J. 1993. ‘Abstract deixis’, Semiotica 95(1/2): 5–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R. P. 2002. ‘Why different, why the same?’, in Meier, R. P., Cormier, K., Quinto-Pozos, D. (eds.), Modality and Structure in Signed and Spoken Languages, pp. 1–25. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Melinger, A. and Levelt, W. J. M. 2004. ‘Gesture and the communicative intention of the speaker’, Gesture 4(2): 119–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menzies, P. 2004. ‘Difference-making in context’, in Collins, J., Hall, N., and Paul, L. (eds.), Causation and Counterfactuals, pp. 139–80. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Metzger, M. 1995. ‘Constructed dialogue and constructed action in American Sign Language’, in Lucas, C. (ed.), Sociolinguistics in Deaf Communities, pp. 255–71. Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Meulen, F. F. 2001. Moving Eyes and Naming Objects. MPI series in psycholinguistics. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. S. 2004. ‘The use of eye tracking in studies of sentence generation’, in Henderson, J. M. and Ferreira, F. (eds.), The Interface of Language, Vision, and Action, pp. 191–212. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. S. and Dobel, C. 2003. ‘Application of eye tracking in speech production research’, in Hyönä, J., Radach, R., and Deubel, H. (eds.), The Mind's Eye: Cognitive and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research, pp. 253–72. Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, A. S. and Meulen, F. F. 2000. ‘Phonological priming effects on speech onset latencies and viewing times in object naming’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 7: 314–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A. M., and Levelt, W. J. M. 1998. ‘Viewing and naming objects: eye movements during noun phrase production’, Cognition 66: B25–B33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Michotte, A. E. 1963 [1946]. The Perception of Causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Michotte, A. E. and Thinès, G. 1963. ‘La causalité perceptive [Perceptual causality]’, Journal de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique 60: 9–36. Reprinted [1991] in G. Thinès, A. Costall and G. Butterworth (eds.), Michotte's Experimental Phenomenology of Perception, pp. 66–87. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. (English translation by the editors.)Google Scholar
Miles, M. 2000. ‘Signing in the Seraglio: Mutes, dwarfs and gestures at the Ottoman Court 1500–1700’, Disability & Society 15(1): 115–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, T. E. and Franklin, D. W. 2005. ‘Impedance control and internal model use during the initial stage of adaptation to novel dynamics in humans’, Journal of Physiology 567: 651–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, T. E., Franklin, D. W., Imamizu, H., and Kawato, M. 2007. ‘Central control of grasp: Manipulation of objects with complex and simple dynamics’, Neuroimage 36: 388–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, G. 1999. ‘Event packaging in BSL discourse’, in Winston, E. (ed.), Storytelling and Conversation: Discourse in Deaf Communities, pp. 27–58. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Morgan, J. L. and Meyer, A. S. 2005. ‘Processing of extrafoveal objects during multiple-object naming’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 31: 428–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Morrison, J. B. and Tversky, B. 2005. ‘Bodies and their parts’, Memory and Cognition, 33: 696–709.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, C. 1994. ‘Semantic structure of motional gestures and lexicalization patterns in Spanish and German descriptions of motion-events’, in Beals, K., Denton, J. M., Knippen, R., Melnar, L., Suzuki, H., and Zeinfeld, E. (eds.), Papers from the Annual Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. The main session, vol. 30, pp. 281–95. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Müller, C. 1998. Redebegleitende Gesten. Kulturgeschichte-Theorie-Sprachvergleich. Berlin: Berlin Verlag Arno Spitz GmbH.Google Scholar
Naigles, L. R. and Hoff-Ginsberg, E. 1998. ‘Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use’, Journal of Child Language 25: 95–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naigles, L. R. and Terrazas, P. 1998. ‘Motion-verb generalizations in English and Spanish: Influences of language and syntax’, Psychological Science 9(5): 363–9.CrossRef
Naigles, L. R., Eisenberg, A. R., Kako, E. T., Highter, M., and McGraw, N. 1998. ‘Speaking of motion: Verb use in English and Spanish’, Language & Cognitive Processes 13(5): 521–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narasimhan, B. and Brown, P. 2008. ‘Getting the INSIDE story: Learning to talk about containment in Tzeltal and Hindi’, in Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (ed.), Routes to Language: Studies in Honor of Melissa Bowerman, pp. 97–132. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Narasimhan, B. and Gullberg, M. 2006. ‘Perspective-shifts in event descriptions in Tamil child language’, Journal of Child Language 33(1): 99–124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narasimhan, B. and Gullberg, M. 2010. ‘The role of input frequency and semantic transparency in the acquisition of verb meaning: evidence from placement verbs in Tamil and Dutch.’ Journal of Child Language doi: 10.1017/S0305 00091 0000164.Google ScholarPubMed
Narasimhan, B., Eisenbeiß, S., and Brown, P. 2007a. ‘Two's company, more is a crowd: the linguistic encoding of multiple-participant events’, Linguistics 45(3): 383–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narasimhan, B., Eisenbeiß, S., and Brown, P. (eds.) 2007b. ‘Special issue on the linguistic encoding of multiple-participant events’, Linguistics 45(3): 383–681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, O. and Sanders, A. F. (eds.) 1996. Handbook of Perception and Action, vol. 3: Attention. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Newman, J. 2002a. ‘A cross-linguistic overview of the posture verbs “sit”, “stand”, and “lie”’, in Newman, J. (ed.), The Linguistics of Sitting, Standing, and Lying, pp. 1–24. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. (ed.). 2002b. The Linguistics of Sitting, Standing, and Lying. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, J. and Rice, S. 2004. ‘Patterns of usage for English SIT, STAND, and LIE: A cognitively inspired exploration in corpus linguistics’, Cognitive Linguistics 15(3): 351–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newport, E. and Supalla, T. 2000. ‘Sign language research at the millennium’, in Emmorey, K. and Lane, H. (eds.), The Signs of Language Revisited. An Anthology to Honor Ursula Bellugi and Edward Klima, pp. 103–14. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Newtson, D. 1973. ‘Attribution and the unit of perception of ongoing behavior’, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 28(1): 28–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newtson, D. and Engquist, G. 1976. ‘The perceptual organization of ongoing behavior’, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 12(5): 436–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyst, V. 2004. ‘Verb series of non-agentive motion in Adamorobe Sign Language (Ghana)’, poster presented at Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research 8, held in Barcelona, Spain, Sept. 30–Oct. 2, 2004.
Nyst, V. 2007. A descriptive analysis of Adamorobe Sign Language (Ghana). Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
Oakes, L. M. 1994. ‘The development of infants’ use of continuity cues in their perception of causality', Developmental Psychology 30: 869–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odlin, T. 2005. ‘Crosslinguistic influence and conceptual transfer: What are the concepts?’, Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 25: 3–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oh, K. J. 2003. Language, cognition, and development: motion events in English and Korean. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
Ohta, K. and Laboissière, R. 2006. ‘Underlying principles of trajectory formation for human movement in dynamical environments’, International Congress Series 1291: 97–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliva, A. and Torralba, A. 2001. ‘Modeling the shape of the scene: A holistic representation of the spatial envelope’, International Journal of Computer Vision 42: 145–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliva, A., Wolfe, J. M., and Arsenio, H. C. 2004. ‘Panoramic search: the interaction of memory and vision in search through a familiar scene’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 30: 1132–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Özyürek, A., Kita, S., Allen, S. E. M., Furman, R., and Brown, A. 2005. ‘How does linguistic framing of events influence co-speech gestures? Insights from cross-linguistic variations and similarities’, Gesture 5(1/2): 219–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özyürek, A., Willems, R. M., Kita, S., and Hagoort, P. 2007. ‘On-line integration of semantic information from speech and gesture: Insights from event-related brain potentials’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19: 605–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, S., Rosch, E., and Chase, P. 1981. ‘Canonical perspective and the perception of objects’, in Long, J. B. and Baddeley, A. D. (eds.), Attention and Performance, IX, pp. 135–51. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Papafragou, A., Massey, C., and Gleitman, L. 2002. ‘Shake, rattle, ’n' roll: The representation of motion in language and cognition', Cognition 84(2): 189–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papaxanthis, C., Pozzo, T., and McIntyre, J. 2005. ‘Kinematic and dynamic processes for the control of pointing movements in humans revealed by short-term exposure to microgravity’, Neuroscience 135: 371–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parsons, P. 1990. Events in the Semantics of English: A Study in Subatomic Semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Pauwels, P. 2000. Put, Set, Lay and Place: A Cognitive Linguistic Approach to Verbal Meaning. München: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. 1999. ‘New approaches to concepts in bilingual memory’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 2(3): 209–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pawley, A. 1987. ‘Encoding events in Kalam and English: different logics for reporting experience’, in Tomlin, R. S. (ed.), Coherence and Grounding in Discourse, pp. 329–60. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pawley, A. 1993. ‘A language which defies description by ordinary means’, in Foley, W. (ed.), The Role of Theory in Language Description, pp. 87–129. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pawley, A. 2008. ‘Compact versus narrative serial verb constructions in Kalam’, in Senft, G. (ed.), Serial Verb Constructions in Austronesian and Papuan Languages, pp. 171–202. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Pawley, A. and Bulmer, R. 2003. A Dictionary of Kalam with Ethnographic Notes. Printout. Department of Linguistics, Research School of Pacific and Asian Studies, Australian National University.Google Scholar
Pawley, A. and Lane, J. 1998. ‘From event sequence to grammar: serial verb constructions in Kalam’, in Siewierska, A. and Jung, S. J. (eds.), Case, Typology and Grammar, pp. 201–27. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pawley, A. and Syder, F. 1983. ‘Two puzzles for linguistic theory: nativelike selection and nativelike fluency’, in Richards, J. and Schmidt, R. (eds.), Language and Communication, pp. 191–225. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Pawley, A. and Syder, F. 2000. ‘The one clause at a time hypothesis’, in Riggenbach, H. (ed.), Perspectives on Fluency, pp. 163–99. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Pederson, E. 1995. ‘Language as context, language as means: Spatial cognition and habitual language use’, Cognitive Linguistics 6(1): 33–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pederson, E., Danziger, E., Levinson, S. C., Kita, S., Senft, G., and Wilkins, D. P. 1998. ‘Semantic typology and spatial conceptualization’, Language 74(3): 557–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penfield, W. and Rasmussen, T. 1950. The Cerebral Cortex of Man. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Perniss, P. 2007a. ‘Achieving spatial coherence in German Sign Language narratives: the use of classifiers and perspective’, Lingua 117: 1315–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P. 2007b. ‘Locative functions of simultaneous perspective constructions in German Sign Language narratives’, in Vermeerbergen, M., Leeson, L., and Crasborn, O. (eds.), Simultaneity in Signed Languages: Form and Function, pp. 27–55. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perniss, P. and Özyürek, A. 2004. ‘Differences in the expressions of spatial relationships in German (DGS) and Turkish (TİD) sign languages’, poster presented at Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research 8, held in Barcelona, Spain, Sept. 30–Oct. 2, 2004.
Perniss, P. and Özyürek, A. 2008. ‘Representations of action, motion and location in sign space: A comparison of German (DGS) and Turkish (TİD) sign language narratives’, in Quer, J. (ed.), Signs of the Time: Selected Papers from TISLR8. Hamburg: Signum Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. 1956. The Child's Conception of Space. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pianesi, F. and Varzi, A. C. 2000. ‘Events and event talk: An introduction’, in Higginbotham, J., Pianesi, F., and Varzi, A. C. (eds.), Speaking of Events, pp. 3–47. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. 1989. Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. 1980. ‘Orienting of attention’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 32: 3–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M.C. 1975. ‘Meaning in visual search’, Science 187: 965–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M. C. and Levi, E. I. 1969. ‘Recognition memory for a rapid sequence of pictures’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 81: 10–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, W. 1997. ‘Perception and action planning’, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9: 129–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, W. and Bridgeman, B. (eds.) 1995. Handbook of Perception and Action, vol. 1: Perception. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F. 2005. ‘Brain mechanisms linking language and action’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6(7): 576–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rayner, K. 1998. ‘Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research’, Psychological Bulletin 124: 372–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reinhart, T. 2002. ‘The theta system: An overview’, Theoretical Linguistics 28: 229–90.Google Scholar
Reinkensmeyer, D. J., Emken, J. L., and Crammer, S. C. 2004. ‘Robotics, motor learning, and neurological recovery’, Annual Review of Biomedical Engineering 6: 497–525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W., Johnson, D., and Boyes-Braem, P. 1976. ‘Basic objects in natural categories’, Cognitive Psychology 8: 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, S. (ed.) 1998a. Events and Grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothstein, S. 1998b. ‘Introduction’, in Rothstein, S. (ed.), Events and Grammar, pp. 1–11. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. 1936. ‘On order in time’, Proceedings of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 32: 216–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. 1948. Human Knowledge. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Ryle, G. 1949. The Concept of Mind. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Sachs, J. 1983. ‘Talking about the there and then: The emergence of displaced reference in parent–child discourse’, in Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children's Language, vol. 4, pp. 1–28. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sakel, J. 2004. A Grammar of Mosetén. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, W. 1994. ‘Causality without counterfactuals’, Philosophy of Science 61: 297–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salmon, W. 1998. Causality and Explanation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasse, H.-J. 2002. ‘Recent activity in the theory of aspect: Accomplishments, achievements, or just non progressive state?’, Linguistic Typology 6(2): 199–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaffer, J. 2000. ‘Causation by disconnection’, Philosophy of Science 67: 285–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Abelson, R. P. 1977. Scripts, Plans, Goals, and Understanding: An Inquiry into Human Knowledge Structures. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schembri, A. 2003. ‘Rethinking “classifiers” in signed languages’, in Emmorey, K. (ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages, pp. 3–34. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schick, B. S. 1990. ‘Classifier predicates in American Sign Language’, International Journal of Sign Linguistics 1(1): 15–40.Google Scholar
Schlottman, A. and Anderson, N. H. 1993. ‘An information integration approach to phenomenal causality’, Memory and Cognition 21(6): 785–801.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholl, B. J. and Tremoulet, P. D. 2000. ‘Perceptual causality and animacy’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 299–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, L., Kushnir, T., and Gopnik, A. 2007. ‘Learning from doing: Intervention and causal inference’, in Gopnick, A. and Schulz, L. (eds.), Causal Learning: Psychology, Philosophy, and Computation, pp. 67–85. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, D. L. 1999. ‘Physical imagery: kinematic versus dynamic models’, Cognitive Psychology 38: 433–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, D. L. and Black, J. B. 1996. ‘Analog imagery in mental model reasoning: Depictive models’, Cognitive Psychology 30: 154–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serra Borneto, C. 1996. ‘Liegen and stehen in German: A study in horizontality and verticality’, in Casad, E. H. (ed.), Cognitive Linguistics in the Redwoods, pp. 459–505. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Seyfeddinipur, M. 2006. Disfluency: Interrupting speech and gesture. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Radboud University, Nijmegen.
Shadmehr, R. and Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. 1994. ‘Adaptive representation of dynamics during learning of a motor task’, Journal of Neuroscience 14: 3208–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shibatani, M. 1976a. The Grammar of Causative Constructions. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shibatani, M. 1976b. ‘The grammar of causative constructions: a conspectus’, in Shibatani, M. (ed.), Syntax and Semantics, vol. 6: The Grammar of Causative Constructions, pp. 1–40. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shultz, T. R. 1982. ‘Rules of causal attribution’, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 47: 1–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinha, C. and Kuteva, T. 1995. ‘Distributed spatial semantics’, Nordic Journal of Linguistics 18(2): 167–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1987. ‘Thinking for speaking’, Berkeley Linguistic Society, BLS 13: 435–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1991. ‘Learning to think for speaking’, Pragmatics 1: 7–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1996a. ‘From “thought and language” to “thinking for speaking”’, in Gumperz, J. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Rethinking Linguistic Relativity, pp. 70–96. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 1996b. ‘Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish’, in Shibatani, M. and Thompson, S. A. (eds.), Grammatical Constructions: Their Form and Meaning, pp. 70–96. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2000. ‘Verbalized events: a dynamic approach to linguistic relativity and determinism’, in Niemeier, S. and Dirven, R. (eds.), Evidence for Linguistic Relativity, pp. 107–38. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2003. ‘Language and thought online: Cognitive consequences of linguistic relativity’, in Gentner, D. and Goldin-Meadow, S. (eds.), Language in Mind: Advances in the Study of Language and Thought, pp. 157–91. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2004. ‘How people move. Discourse effects of linguistic typology’, in Moder, C. L. and Martinovic-Zic, A. (eds.), Discourse Across Languages and Cultures, pp. 195–210. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, D. I. 2006. ‘What makes manner of motion salient?: Explorations of linguistic typology, discourse, and cognition’, in Hickmann, M. and Robert, S. (eds.), Space in Languages: Linguistic Systems and Cognitive Categories, pp. 59–81. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. 2005. Causal Models: How People Think about the World and its Alternatives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Barbey, K. A., and Hotaling, J. M. 2009. ‘A causal model theory of the meaning of cause, enable, and prevent’, Cognitive Science 33: 21–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. S. 1978. ‘Jespersen's “move and change” class and causative verbs in English’, in Jazayery, M. A., Palome, E. C., and Winter, W. (eds.), Linguistic and Literary Studies in Honor of Archibald A. Hill, vol. 2: Descriptive Linguistics, pp. 101–9. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Smith, C. S. 1991. The Parameter of Aspect: Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, G. 1996. ‘Causation, adicity and lexical aspect’, in Przezdziecki, M. and Whaley, L. (eds.), ESCOL '95, pp. 299–307. Ithaca, NY: CLC.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. S., Phillips, A. T., and Woodward, A. L. 1995. ‘Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action', in Sperber, D., Premack, D., and Premack, A. (eds.), Causal Cognition: A Multidisciplinary Debate, pp. 44–78. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spellman, B. A. and Mandel, D. R. 1999. ‘When possibility informs reality: Counterfactual thinking as a cue to causality’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 8: 120–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spellman, B. A., Kincannon, A. P., and Stose, S. J. 2005. ‘The relation between counterfactual and causal reasoning’, in Mandel, D. R., Hilton, D. J., and Catellani, P. (eds.), The Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking, pp. 28–43. London: Routledge Research.Google Scholar
Staden, M. and Reesink, G. 2008. ‘A functional approach to verb serialization’, in Senft, G. (ed.), Serial Verb Constructions in Austronesian and Papuan Languages, pp. 17–54. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Staden, M., Bowerman, M., and Verhelst, M. 2006. ‘Some properties of spatial description in Dutch’, in Levinson, S. C. and Wilkins, D. P. (eds.), Grammars of Space. Explorations in Cognitive Diversity, pp. 475–511. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staden, M., Senft, G., Enfield, N., and Bohnemeyer, J. 2001. ‘Staged events’, in Enfield, N. (ed.), ‘Manual’ for the Field Season 2001, pp. 115–25. Nijmegen: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. 1959. Individuals. London: Methuen and Co. Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stutterheim, C. and Klein, W. 2002. ‘Quaestio and L-perspectivation’, in Graumann, C. F. and Kallmeyer, W. (eds.), Perspective and Perspectivation in Discourse, pp. 59–88. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stutterheim, C. and Nüse, R. 2003. ‘Processes of conceptualisation in language production’, Linguistics (Special Issue: Perspectives in language production), 41: 851–81.Google Scholar
Stutterheim, C., Carroll, M., and Klein, W. 2003. ‘Two ways of construing complex temporal structures’, in Lenz, F. (ed.), Deictic Conceptualization of Space, Time and Person, pp. 97–133. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stutterheim, C., Nüse, R., and Murcia-Serra, J. 2002. ‘Cross-linguistic differences in the conceptualization of events’, in Hasselgård, H., Johansson, S., Behrens, B., and Fabricius-Hansen, C. (eds.), Information Structure in a Cross-linguistic Perspective, pp. 179–98. Amsterdam: Rodopi.Google Scholar
Supalla, T. 1986. ‘The classifier system in American Sign Language’, in Craig, C. (ed.), Noun Classification and Categorization, pp. 181–213. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Supalla, T. and Webb, R. 1995. ‘The grammar of international sign’, in Emmorey, K. and Reilly, J. (eds.), Language, Gesture, and Space, pp. 333–54. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 1985. ‘Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms’, in Shopen, T. (ed.), Language Typology and Syntactic Description, vol. 3: Grammatical Categories and the Lexicon, pp. 57–149. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 1988. ‘Force dynamics in language and cognition’, Cognitive Science 12: 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. 1991. ‘Path to realization: A typology of event conflation’, Proceedings of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 17: 480–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. 2000a. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, vol. I: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2000b. Toward a Cognitive Semantics, vol. II: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2003a. ‘Concept structuring systems in language’, in Tomasello, M. (ed.), The New Psychology of Language. Cognitive and Functional Approaches to Language Structure, vol. 2, pp. 15–46. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2003b. ‘The representation of spatial structure in spoken and signed language’, in Emmorey, K. (ed.), Perspectives on Classifier Constructions in Sign Languages, pp. 169–96. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. 2008. ‘Aspects of attention in language’, in Ellis, N. C. and Robinson, P. (eds.), Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics and Second Language Acquisition, pp. 27–38. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Taylor, H. A. and Tversky, B. 1992. ‘Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions’, Journal of Memory and Language 31: 261–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tenny, C. and Pustejovsky, J. 2000. ‘A history of events in linguistic theory’, in Tenny, C. and Pustejovsky, J. (eds.), Events as Grammatical Objects: The Converging Perspectives of Lexical Semantics and Syntax, pp. 3–37. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Thinès, G., Costall, A., and Butterworth, G. 1991. Michotte's Experimental Phenomenology of Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Thorpe, S., Fize, D., and Marlot, C. 1996. ‘Speed of processing in the human visual system’, Nature 381: 520–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorpe, S., Gegenfurtner, K. R., Fabre-Thorpe, M., and Bülthoff, H. H. 2001. ‘Detection of animals in natural images using far peripheral vision’, European Journal of Neuroscience 14: 869–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomlin, R. S. 1997. ‘Mapping conceptual representations into linguistic representations: the role of attention in grammar’, in Nuyts, J. and Pederson, E. (eds.), Language and Conceptualization, pp. 162–89. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treisman, A. and Gelade, G. 1980. ‘A feature-integration theory of attention’, Cognitive Psychology 12: 97–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B. and Hemenway, K. 1984. ‘Objects, parts, and categories’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 113: 169–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, B. and Tuchin, M. 1989. ‘A reconciliation of evidence on eyewitness testimony: Comments on McCloskey and Zaragoza (1985)’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 118: 86–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B., Lee, P., and Zacks, J. M. 2004. ‘Events by hand and feet’, Spatial Cognition and Computation 4: 5–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, B., Zacks, J. M., and Hard, B. M. 2008. ‘The structure of experience’, in Shipley, T. and Zacks, J. M. (eds.), Understanding Events: How Humans See, Represent, and Act on Events, pp. 436–65. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oosten, J. 1984. ‘Sitting, standing and lying in Dutch: A cognitive approach to the distribution of the verbs zitten, staan, and liggen’, in Oosten, J. and Snapper, J. (eds.), Dutch Linguistics at Berkeley, pp. 137–60. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Rullen, R. and Thorpe, S. J. 2001. ‘The time course of visual processing: From early perception to decision-making’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13: 454–61.Google Scholar
Valin, R. D. Jr. and LaPolla, R. J. 1997. Syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vendler, Z. 1957. ‘Verbs and times’, Philosophical Review 56: 143–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verfaillie, K. and Daems, A. 1996. ‘The priority of the agent in visual event perception: On the cognitive basis of grammatical agent–patient asymmetries’, Cognitive Linguistics 7(2): 131–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhagen, A. and Kemmer, S. 1997. ‘Interaction and causation: Causative constructions in modern standard Dutch’, Journal of Pragmatics 27: 61–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogel, H. 1999. ‘Geschichte der Gehörlosenbildung’, in Beecken, A., Keller, J., Prillwitz, S., and Zienert, H. (eds.), Grundkurs Deutsche Gebärdensprache, Stufe I, Arbeitsbuch, pp. 46–9. Hamburg: Signum Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., and Pizzuto, E. 2005. ‘Gesture and the emergence and development of language’, in Tomasello, M. and Slobin, D. I. (eds.), Beyond Nature–Nurture: Essays in Honor of Elizabeth Bates, pp. 3–40. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Warren, W. H.. and Shaw, R. E. (eds.) 1985. Persistence and Change. Proceedings of the First International Conference on Event Perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Weinstein, S. 1968. ‘Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality’, in Kenshalo, D. R. (ed.), The Skin Senses, pp. 195–222. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
White, P. A. 1999. ‘Towards a causal realist theory of causal understanding’, American Journal of Psychology 112: 605–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, P. A. 2006. ‘Theoretical notes: A causal asymmetry’, Psychological Review 113: 132–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B. L. 1956. Language, Thought, and Reality, ed. by Carroll, J. B.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. 1980. Lingua Mentalis. Sydney: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. 1998. ‘Visual search’, in Pashler, H. (ed.), Attention, pp. 13–73. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Klempen, N., and Dahlen, K. 2000. ‘Postattentive vision’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 26: 693–716.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. 2003. ‘Direct causation in the linguistic coding and individuation of causal events’, Cognition 88: 1–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. 2007. ‘Representing causation’, Journal of Experimental Psychology, General 136: 82–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. 2008. ‘Dynamics and the perception of causal events’, in Shipley, T. and Zacks, J. (eds.), Understanding Events, pp. 555–88. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolff, P. and Zettergren, M. 2002. ‘A vector model of causal meaning’, in Gray, W. D. and Schunn, C. D. (eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp. 944–9. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woodward, J. 2003. Making Things Happen. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Woodward, J. 2006. ‘Sensitive and insensitive causation’, Philosophical Review 115: 1–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. 2007. ‘Interventionist theories of causation in psychological perspective’, in Gopnik, A. and Schulz, L. (eds.), Causal Learning: Psychology, Philosophy, and Computation, pp. 19–36. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wray, A. 2002. Formulaic Language and the Lexicon. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wunderlich, D. (ed.) 2006. Advances in the Theory of the Lexicon. Berlin: de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoshioka, K. and Kellerman, E. 2006. ‘Gestural introduction of Ground reference in L2 narrative discourse’, International Review of Applied Linguistics 44(2): 171–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, J. M. 2004. ‘Using movement and intentions to understand simple events’, Cognitive Science 28: 979–1008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, J. M. and Swallow, K. M. 2007. ‘Event segmentation’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 16: 80–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zacks, J. M. and Tversky, B. 2001. ‘Event structure in perception and conception’, Psychological Bulletin 127: 3–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zacks, J. M., Tversky, B., and Iyer, G. 2001. ‘Perceiving, remembering and communicating structure in events’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 136: 29–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zacks, J. M., Kumar, S., Abrams, R. A., and Mehta, R. 2009. ‘Using movement and intentions to understand human activity’, Cognition 112: 201–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeshan, U. 2002. ‘Sign language in Turkey: The story of a hidden language’, Journal of Turkic Languages, 6(2): 229–74.Google Scholar
Zwitserlood, I. 2003. Classifying hand configurations in Nederlandse Gebarentaal. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Utrecht, LOT.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Jürgen Bohnemeyer, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Eric Pederson, University of Oregon
  • Book: Event Representation in Language and Cognition
  • Online publication: 01 March 2011
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511782039.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Jürgen Bohnemeyer, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Eric Pederson, University of Oregon
  • Book: Event Representation in Language and Cognition
  • Online publication: 01 March 2011
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511782039.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Jürgen Bohnemeyer, University at Buffalo, State University of New York, Eric Pederson, University of Oregon
  • Book: Event Representation in Language and Cognition
  • Online publication: 01 March 2011
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511782039.012
Available formats
×