Book contents
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of Contributors
- Preface
- Part I Academic Cheating
- Part II Academic Excuses and Fairness
- Part III Authorship and Credit
- Part IV Confidentiality’s Limits
- Part V Data Analysis, Reporting, and Sharing
- Part VI Designing Research
- 34 Complete or Incomplete, That Is the Question
- 35 “Getting It Right” Can Also Be Wrong
- 36 Commentary to Part VI
- Part VII Fabricating Data
- Part VIII Human Subjects
- Part IX Personnel Decisions
- Part X Reviewing and Editing
- Part XI Science for Hire and Conflict of Interest
- Epilogue Why Is Ethical Behavior Challenging?
- Index
35 - “Getting It Right” Can Also Be Wrong
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 05 February 2015
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of Contributors
- Preface
- Part I Academic Cheating
- Part II Academic Excuses and Fairness
- Part III Authorship and Credit
- Part IV Confidentiality’s Limits
- Part V Data Analysis, Reporting, and Sharing
- Part VI Designing Research
- 34 Complete or Incomplete, That Is the Question
- 35 “Getting It Right” Can Also Be Wrong
- 36 Commentary to Part VI
- Part VII Fabricating Data
- Part VIII Human Subjects
- Part IX Personnel Decisions
- Part X Reviewing and Editing
- Part XI Science for Hire and Conflict of Interest
- Epilogue Why Is Ethical Behavior Challenging?
- Index
Summary
My student and I sat at my desk discussing the results – or rather the lack thereof – of our recent lab study. We had already conducted one experiment that provided support for our hypothesis, which we regarded as new and interesting. That first study, however, involved purely attitudinal measures, and the obvious next step was to assess actual behavior. We selected a behavioral task that appeared in the literature, made a few changes to our experimental protocol, and ran the study with ample participants per cell. Lo and behold, we found no support for our hypothesis. So there we were in my office, discussing our nonsignificant results and our next step. As good Popperians, we remained true to our hypothesis; we discussed the next study to be conducted, one that we hoped would make use of a “better” task to assess our participants’ behavior – that is, a task that would produce our expected effect. This time we would “get it right.”
We didn’t seriously question our hypothesis; rather we scrutinized features of the experiment. We went back to the literature to seek an alternative behavioral measure and found one we believed would serve our purpose better. We are now conducting this revamped study. Of course the purpose of research should be ascertaining whether there is a true, reliable effect; but clearly our purpose was to find an effect, and in particular the effect we hypothesized. If we got it right in the current study and find our hypothesized effect, we will likely write up the two confirmatory studies, one assessing attitudes and the other behavior, and submit our paper for publication. The null effects of the intermediary experiment will not be discussed or mentioned, but will be ignored and forgotten. And null findings in this newest study wouldn’t necessarily preclude revising our protocol once again to “get it right.”
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- Ethical Challenges in the Behavioral and Brain SciencesCase Studies and Commentaries, pp. 105 - 107Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 2015