Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T13:58:29.587Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - From paragon to pariah: Cross-cultural perspectives on attitudes to dogs

from PART III - DOG-HUMAN INTERACTIONS

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 December 2016

James Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
James Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Human attitudes to non-human animals (henceforth “animals”) are influenced by a variety of factors. For example, animals that are perceived to be economically or instrumentally beneficial to human interests are generally, though not invariably, viewed more positively than those that are seen to be detrimental. Thus, honey bees that make honey and pollinate crops are regarded more positively than cockroaches, even though bees can sting – sometimes fatally – and cockroaches cannot. Certain animals may also possess inherent characteristics of appearance or behavior that humans find appealing or repellant on a purely emotional level, independent of the animal's instrumental value. Butterflies, for instance, tend to be liked for purely aesthetic reasons, while snakes are commonly loathed, despite the latter playing an important role in the control of agricultural pests. Animals are also “culturally constructed” in the sense that they acquire a wealth of symbolic and metaphorical associations and meanings that are peculiar to particular cultural settings. For example, the “sacred” status of cows in India is a cultural construct derived from Hindu religion that contrasts markedly with attitudes to cattle in, say, Europe or North America (Serpell, 1996, 2004).

Western attitudes to animals are colored by a strong sense of human exceptionalism that has its roots in the traditional Judaeo-Christian worldview. According to this perspective, humans and other animals were produced by separate acts of divine creation; the former to rule over the latter, and the latter to serve the exclusive interests of the former (Midgley, 1983; Serpell, 1996, 2005; Thomas, 1983). Despite such traditions, it is nonetheless clear that the distinction between human and non-human can become blurred under certain circumstances, such as when human victims of political or racial oppression find themselves summarily reclassified as “animals” preparatory to having their legal and moral rights ignored or denied (see e.g. Arluke & Sax, 1992). Occasionally, the barrier can also be penetrated from the opposite direction. Just as people are sometimes classified as animals, so animals – for one reason or another – may be categorized and treated as persons. In the Middle Ages, dangerous animals and vermin were occasionally tried, tortured and executed for committing “crimes,” as if they were guilty of malicious intent and, like people, could be held morally accountable for their actions (Dinzelbacher, 2002; Evans, 1906; Girgen, 2003).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Domestic Dog
Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People
, pp. 300 - 316
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

American Society for the Protection of Animals (ASPCA) (2014). [Online]. Available: www.aspca.org/about-us/faq/pet-statistics [accessed November 19, 2014].
Appleby, D. & Pluijmakers, J. (2003). Separation anxiety in dogs: the function of homeostasis in its development and treatment. Veterinary Clinics Small Animal Practice, 33: 321–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arluke, A. (1988). Sacrificial symbolism in animal experimentation: object or pet? Anthrozoös, 2: 98–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arluke, A. (1994). Managing emotions in an animal shelter. In Animals and Human Society: Changing Perspectives, eds. Manning, A. & Serpell, J. A.. London: Routledge, pp. 145–65.Google Scholar
Arluke, A. & Sax, B. (1992). Understanding Nazi animal protection and the Holocaust. Anthrozoös, 5: 6–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arman, K. (2007). A new direction for kennel club regulations and breed standards. Canadian Veterinary Journal, 48: 953–965.Google ScholarPubMed
Asher, L., Diesel, G., Summers, J. F., McGreevy, P. D. & Collins, L. M. (2009). Inherited defects in pedigree dogs. Part 1: Disorders related to breed standards. The Veterinary Journal, 182: 402–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avieli, N. (2011). Dog meat politics in a Vietnamese town. Ethnology, 50: 59–78.Google Scholar
Beck, A. M. & Katcher, A. H. (1996). Between Pets and People: The Importance of Animal Companionship. West Lafayette, IN: Purdue University Press.Google Scholar
Beier, U. (1959). The Yoruba attitude to dogs. Odu, 7: 31–7.Google Scholar
Bökönyi, S. (1974). History of Domestic Mammals in Central and Eastern Europe. Budapest: Académiai Kaidó.Google Scholar
Broch, H. B. (2008). Gender and Matinen dogs. Asian Anthropology, 7: 57–77 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bueler, L. (1974). Wild Dogs of The World. London: Constable.Google Scholar
Burkardt, V. R. (1960). Chinese Creeds and Customs, vol. 3. Hong Kong: South China Morning Post Ltd.Google Scholar
Burt, M. R. (1988). The animal as Alter Ego: cruelty, altruism, and the work of art. In Animals and People Sharing the World, ed. Rowan, A. N.. Hanover, NH: University Press of New England, pp. 117–35.Google Scholar
Centers for Disease Control (2014). [Online]. Available: http://emergency.cdc.gov/disasters/lightning/index.asp [accessed November 14, 2014].
Cipriani, L. (1966). The Andaman Islanders. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, J. (1994). Hot dogs: comestible canids in preclassic Maya culture at Cuello, Belize. Journal of Archaeological Science, 21: 819–26 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collier, S. (2006). Breed-specific legislation and the pit bull terrier: are the laws justified? Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 1: 17–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1993). Gaps in the mind. In The Great Ape Project, eds. Cavalieri, P. & Singer, P.. London: Fourth Estate, pp. 80–7.Google Scholar
Diamond, J. M. (1993). The third chimpanzee. In The Great Ape Project, eds. Cavalieri, P. & Singer, P.. London: Fourth Estate, pp. 88–101.Google Scholar
Dinzelbacher, P. (2002). Animal trials: a multidisciplinary approach. Journal of Interdisciplinary History, 32: 405–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1966). Purity and Danger: An Analysis of the Concepts of Pollution and Taboo. New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Downs, J. F. (1960). Domestication: an examination of the changing social relationships between man and animals. Kroeber Anthropological Society Papers, 22: 18–67.Google Scholar
Driver, H. E. & Massey, W. C. (1957). Comparative studies of North American Indians. Transactions of the American Philosophical Society, 42: 165–456.Google Scholar
Dumont, J. P. (1976). Under the Rainbow: Nature and Supernature among the Panaré Indians. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Durrenburger, E. P. (1977). Of Lisu dogs and Lisu spirits. Folklore, 88: 61–3.Google Scholar
Ellen, R. (1999). Categories of animality and canine abuse: Exploring contradictions in Nuala social relationships with dogs. Anthropos, 94: 57–68.Google Scholar
Elmendorf, W. W. & Kroeber, A. L. (1960). The structure of Twana culture with comparative notes on the structure of Yurok culture. Washington University Research Studies, 28(2). Pullman, WA: Washington State University.Google Scholar
Evans, E.P. (1906). The Criminal Prosecution and Capital Punishment of Animals. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1967). Effects of early experience on the development of inter and intra-specific social relationships in the dog. Animal Behaviour, 15: 377–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, B. (1965). Die Rolle des Hunde im Africanischen Kulturen. Wiesbaden: Franz Steiner Verlag.Google Scholar
Girgen, J. (2003). The historical and contemporary prosecution and punishment of animals. Animal Law, 9: 97–133.Google Scholar
Gottlieb, A. (1986). Dog: ally or traitor? Mythology, cosmology, and society among the Beng of Ivory Coast. American Ethnologist, 13: 447–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108: 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, T. (1965). Three “secret” communication systems between Borneo nomads (and their dogs). Journal of the Malay Branch of the Royal Asiatic Society, 38: 67–86.Google Scholar
Hole, F. & Wyllie, C. (2007) The oldest depictions of canines and a possible early breed of dog in Iran. Paléorient, 33: 175–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hose, C. & McDougall, M. B. (1912). The Pagan Tribes of Borneo. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Huntingford, G. W. B. 1955. The economic life of the Dorobo. Anthropos, 50: 602–34.Google Scholar
Ikeya, K. (1994). Hunting with dogs among the San in the Central Kalahari. African Study Monographs, 15: 119–34.Google Scholar
Ishige, N. (1977). Roasting dog (or a substitute) in an earth oven: an unusual method of preparation from Ponape. In The Anthropologist's Cookbook, ed. Kuper, J.. New York: Universe Books, pp. 204–5.Google Scholar
James, J. (2006). The dog tribe. The Southern Anthropologist, 32: 17–46.Google Scholar
Jesse, G.R. (1866). Researches into the History of the British Dog, vol. I. London: Robert Hardwicke.Google Scholar
Jordan, J. W. (1975). An ambivalent relationship: dog and human in the folk culture of the rural south. Appalachian Journal, Spring: 238–48.
Julius, H., Beetz, A., Kotrschal, K., Turner, D. C. & Uvnäs-Moberg, K. (2012). Attachment to Pets. New York: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Jung, C. (1959). The Archetypes and the Collective Unconscious. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Kohn, E. (2007). How dogs dream: Amazonian natures and the politics of transspecies engagement. American Ethnologist, 34: 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koster, J. (2008). Hunting with dogs in Nicaragua: an optimal foraging approach. Current Anthropology, 49: 935–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koster, J. (2009). Hunting dogs in the lowland tropics. Journal of Anthropological Research, 65: 575–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The! Kung San: Men, Women and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lumholtz, C. (1989). Among Cannibals. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Luomala, K. (1960). The native dog in the Polynesian system of values. In Culture in History, ed. Diamond, S.. New York: Columbia University Press, pp. 190–240.Google Scholar
Menache, S. (1997). Dogs: God's worst enemies? Society and Animals, 5: 23–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Midgley, M. (1983). Animals and Why They Matter. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Mooney, M. M. (1975). George Catlin Letters and Notes on the North American Indians. New York: Clarkson N. Potter.Google Scholar
Nivedita, The Sister & Coomaraswamy, A. K. (1913). Myths of the Hindus and Buddhists. London: Harrap & Co.Google Scholar
Oberholtzer, C. (2002). Fleshing out the evidence: from archaic dog burials to historic dog feasts. Ontario Archaeology, 73: 3–14.Google Scholar
Olowo Ojoade, J. (1990). Nigerian cultural attitudes to the dog. In Signifying Animals: Human Meaning in the Natural World, ed. Willis, R. G.. London: Unwin Hyman, pp. 215–21.Google Scholar
Ortolani, A., Vernooij, H. & Coppinger, R. (2009). Ethiopian village dogs: behavioural responses to stranger's approach. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 119: 210–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osgood, C. (1951). The Koreans and their Culture. New York: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Osgood, C. (1975). The Chinese: A Study of a Hong Kong Community. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.Google Scholar
Patronek, G.J., Sacks, J.J., Delise, K.M., Cleary, D.V. & Marder, A. R. (2013). Co-occurrence of potentially preventable factors in 256 dog bite-related fatalities in the United States (2000–2009). JAVMA, 243: 1726–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulson, R. (1979). Popular and Polite Art in the Age of Hogarth and Fielding. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Perin, C. (1981). Dogs as symbols in human development. In Interrelations between People and Pets, ed. Fogle, B.. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas, pp. 68–88.Google Scholar
Podberscek, A. L. (2009). Good to pet and eat: the keeping and consuming of dogs and cats in South Korea. Journal of Social Issues, 65: 615–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podberscek, A. L., Hsu, Y. & Serpell, J. A. (1999). Evaluation of clomipramine as an adjunct to behavioural therapy in the treatment of separation-related problems in dogs. Veterinary Record, 145: 365–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, W. K. & Powers, M. N. (1986). Putting on the dog. Natural History, 2: 6–16.Google Scholar
Roe, P. G. (1993). The domesticated jaguar: dog symbolism in the lowlands. Proceedings of the 15th International Congress on Caribbean Archaeology, San Juan, Puerto Rico, pp. 2–42.
Sahlins, M. (1976). Culture and Practical Reason. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. (1963). The process of primary socialization in canine and human infants. Monographs of the Society for Research on Child Development, 28: 1–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P. (1968). Evolution and domestication of the dog. Evolutionary Biology, 2: 243–75.Google Scholar
Seligmann, C. G. & Seligmann, B. A. (1911). The Veddas. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (1985). Best friend or worst enemy: cross-cultural variation in attitudes to the dog. In The Human–Pet Relationship. Vienna: IEMT – Institute for Interdisciplinary Research on the Human–Pet Relationship.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (1996). In the Company of Animals, edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (2004). Factors influencing attitudes to animals and their welfare. Animal Welfare, 13: S145–51.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (2005). Animals and religion: Towards a unifying theory. In The Human-Animal Relationship, eds. Jong, F. de & Bos, R. Van den. Assen, Netherlands: Royal Van Gorcum, pp. 9–22.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (2009). Having our dogs and eating them too: Why animals are a social issue. Journal of Social Issues, 65: 633–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (2013). Domestication and history of the cat. In The Domestic Cat: The Biology of Its Behaviour, edition, eds. Turner, D. C. & Bateson, P. P. G.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 83–100.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. & Paul, E. S. (1994). Pets and the development of positive attitudes to animals. In Animals and Human Society: Changing Perspectives, eds. Manning, A. & Serpell, J. A.. London: Routledge, pp. 127–44.Google Scholar
Simoons, F. J. (1996). Dogflesh eating by humans in sub-Saharan Africa. Ecology of Food and Nutrition, 34: 251–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, M. (1978). Pygmies and their dogs: a note on culturally constituted defence mechanisms. Ethos, 6: 270–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, B. P. & Litchfield, C. A. (2009). A review of the relationship between indigenous Australians, dingoes (Canis dingo) and domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Anthrozoös, 22: 111–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Summers, J. F., Diesel, G., Asher, L. McGreevy, P. D. & Collins, L.M. (2010) Inherited defects in pedigree dogs. Part 2: Disorders that are not related to breed standards. The Veterinary Journal, 183: 39–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tambiah, J. S. (1969). Animals are good to think and good to prohibit. Ethnology, 8: 423–59 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, K. (1983). Man and the Natural World: Changing Attitudes in England 1500–1800. London: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Titcomb, M. (1969). Dog and Man in the Ancient Pacific with Special Attention to Hawaii. Honolulu, HI: Bernice P. Bishop Museum Special Publications 59.Google Scholar
White, D. G. (1991). Myths of the Dog-Man. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×