Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T14:23:18.197Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Evolution of working dogs

from PART I - ORIGINS AND EVOLUTION

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 December 2016

Kathryn Lord
Affiliation:
School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, MA, USA
Richard A. Schneider
Affiliation:
Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, University of California at San Francisco, CA, USA
Raymond Coppinger
Affiliation:
School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, MA, USA
James Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Get access

Summary

Introduction

When we think of dogs, we tend to think of animals that were selected for behavior performed in the service of people. Dogs pull sleds, guard property, herd sheep, guide the blind, track and retrieve game, and so on. We also think of dogs in terms of breeds, and often try to identify the breeds that make up some mongrel, as if all dogs had unadulterated, purebred ancestry. Many see their favorite breed woven into the Bayeux Tapestry or carved into the walls of an ancient tomb. Some think of breeds as if they were ancient species, separately derived from different strains of wolves, jackals or even coyotes. And people are often amazed at the many different sizes, colors, shapes, faces, and behaviors dogs come in.

Dogs’ practical service to people is frequently overstated. Of the billion or so dogs in the world, only a tiny percentage of them work or hunt. Pure breeds of dogs for the most part are modern inventions (Larson et al., 2012).

Frequently it is stated that the breeds were “selected” to perform some specific behavior, implying that changes have come about through a gradual accumulation of traits, by a process similar to natural selection. Darwin (1858) used domestic animals as an example of “artificial” selection analogous to natural selection. However, discoveries in the last hundred years have suggested other evolutionary mechanisms underlying the morphological and behavioral conformation of domesticated animals.

This chapter distils our observations and experiments on the subject of breed-specific behavior. It is divided into two sections. Section 4.2 focuses on the functional morphology and behavior of three “types” of working dogs. First are the livestock guarding dogs, which evolved in pastoral societies, where the only selection by humans is for behavior designed to repel or eliminate animals they don't like. Second are the sled dogs, which are bred to pull. Any animal can be taught to pull, but in sled dogs there is intense selection for a superior conformation that can efficiently perform the task. Third are the livestock herding dogs, bred to conduct livestock. There is little selection for physical conformation in herding dogs, but rather intense selection for a behavioral conformation including attention to the quality, frequency, and sequencing of specific motor patterns.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Domestic Dog
Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People
, pp. 42 - 66
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alberch, P. (1982). The generative and regulatory roles of development in evolution. In Environmental Adaptation and Evolution, eds. Mossakowski, D. & Roth, G., pp. 19–36. New York: Gustav Fischer.Google Scholar
Arons, C. D. & Shoemaker, W. J. (1992). The distributions of catecholamines and beta-endorphin in the brains of three behaviorally distinct breeds of dogs and their F1 hybrids. Brain Research, 594: 31–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belyaev, D. K. (1979). Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. Journal of Heredity, 70: 301–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bemis, W. E. (1984). Paedomorphosis and the evolution of the Dipnoi. Paleobiology, 10: 293–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, H. L. & Green, J. S. (1985). Navajo use of mixed-breed dogs for management of predators. Journal of Range Management, 38: 11–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronner-Fraser, M. (1994). Neural crest cell formation and migration in the developing embryo. The Journal of the Federation of American Societies for Experimental Biology, 8: 699–706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The Genesis of Animal Play. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cairns, R. B. & Werboff, J. (1967). Behavior development in the dog: an interspecies analysis. Science, 158: 1070–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, L. (1977). The World of Sled Dogs. New York: Howell Book House.Google Scholar
Coppinger, L. & Coppinger, R. P. (1982). Livestock-guarding dogs that wear sheep's clothing. Smithsonian Magazine, April, 64–73.
Coppinger, L. & Coppinger, R. P. (1993). Dogs for herding and guarding livestock. In Livestock Handling and Transport, ed. Grandin, T.. Cambridge, MA: CAB International, pp. 179–96.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P., Coppinger, L., Langeloh, G., Gettler, L. & Lorenz, J. (1988). A decade of use of livestock guarding dogs. In Proceedings of the Vertebrate Pest Conference, Vol. 13, eds. Crabb, A. C. & Marsh, R. E.. Davis, CA: University of California, pp. 209–14.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P. & Coppinger, L. (2001). Dogs: A Startling New Understanding of Canine Origin, Behavior & Evolution. New York: Scribner.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P. & Coppinger, L. (2016). What is a Dog? Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, R. P. & Feinstein, M. (2015). How Dogs Work. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, R. P., Glendinning, J., Torop, E., Matthay, C, Sutherland, M. & Smith, C. (1987). Degree of behavioral neoteny differentiates canid polymorphs. Ethology, 75: 89–108.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P. & Schneider, R. (1995). Evolution of working dogs. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People, ed. Serpell, J.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 21–47.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P. & Smith, C. K. (1990). A model for understanding the evolution of mammalian behavior. In Current Mammalogy, Vol. 2, ed. Genoways, H.. New York: Plenum, pp. 335–74.Google Scholar
Coppinger, R. P., Smith, C. K. & Miller, L. (1985). Observations on why mongrels may make effective livestock protecting dogs. Journal of Range Management, 38: 560–1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, R. P., Spector, L. & Miller, L. (2010). What if anything is a wolf? In The World of Wolves: New Perspectives on Ecology, Behaviour and Management, eds. Musiani, M., Boitani, L. & Paquet, P.. Calgary, Canada: University of Calgary Press, pp. 41–67.Google Scholar
Couly, G. F., Coltey, P., Eichmann, A. & Le Douarin, N. M. (1995). The angiogenic potentials of the cephalic mesoderm and the origin of brain and head blood vessels. Mechanisms of Development, 53: 97–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Couly, G. F., Coltey, P. M. & Le Douarin, N. M. (1992). The developmental fate of the cephalic mesoderm in quail-chick chimera. Development, 114: 1–15.Google Scholar
Couly, G. F., Coltey, P. M. & Le Douarin, N. M. (1993). The triple origin of skull in higher vertebrates: a study in quail-chick chimeras. Development, 117: 409–29.Google ScholarPubMed
Couly, G. F., & Le Douarin, N. M. (1988). The fate map of the cephalic neural primordium at the presomitic to the 3-somite stage in the avian embryo. Development, 103: 101–13.Google ScholarPubMed
Cramer, S. F. (1991). The origin of epidermal melanocytes. Implications for the histogenesis of nevi and melanomas. Archives of Pathology & Laboratory Medicine, 115: 115–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Crockford, S. J. (2002). Animal domestication and heterochronic speciation. In Human Evolution through Developmental Change, eds. Minughpurvis, N. & McNamara, K. J.. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press, pp. 122–53.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1858). Abstract of a Letter from C. Darwin, Esq., to Prof. Asa Gray, Boston, U.S. dated Down, September 5th, 1857. Proceedings of the Linnean Society of London, Zoology, 3: 50–3.Google Scholar
Drake, A. G. (2011). Dispelling dog dogma: an investigation of heterochrony in dogs using 3D geometric morphometric analysis of skull shape. Evolution & Development, 13: 204–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eames, B. F. & Schneider, R. A. (2005). Quail-duck chimeras reveal spatiotemporal plasticity in molecular and histogenic programs of cranial feather development. Development, 132: 1499–509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eames, B. F. & Schneider, R. A. (2008). The genesis of cartilage size and shape during development and evolution. Development 135: 3947–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eldredge, N. & Gould, S. J. (1972). Punctuated equilibria: an alternative to phyletic gradualism. In Models in Paleobiology, ed. Schopf, T. J. M.. San Francisco, CA: Freeman Cooper, pp. 82–115.Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. (1967). Observations on the behavioral development of a hand-reared male timber wolf. American Zoologist, 7: 339–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fentress, J. C. & McLoed, P. J. (1986). Motor patterns in development. In Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Vol. 8 Developmental Psychobiology and Developmental Neurobiology, ed. Blass, E. M.. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 35–97.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1964). The ontogeny of behaviour and neurologic responses in the dog. Animal Behaviour, 12: 301–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1965). Canine Behavior. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Fox, M. (1969). Behavioral effects of rearing dogs with cats during the “critical period of socialization.” Behaviour, 35: 273–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1971). Integrative Development of Brain and Behavior in the Dog. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1975). Behavior genetics of F1 and F2 coyote-dog hybrids. Applied Animal Ethology, 1: 185–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1978). The Dog: Its Domestication and Behavior. New York: Garland STPM Press.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W. & Bekoff, M. (1975). The behaviour of dogs. In The Behaviour of Domestic Animals, edition, ed. Hafez, E. S. E.. London: Baillière Tindall, pp. 370–409.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W., Halperin, S., Wise, A. & Kohn, E. (1976). Species and hybrid differences in frequencies of play and agonistic actions in Canids. Zeitchrift fur Tierpsychologie, 40: 194–209.Google ScholarPubMed
Frank, H. & Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8: 507–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, D. G., King, J. A. & Elliot, O. (1960). Critical period in the social development of dogs. Science, 133: 1016–17.Google Scholar
Fuller, J. L. & Dubois, E. M. (1962). The behavior of dogs. In The Behavior of Domestic Animals, ed. Hafez, E. S.. Baltimore, MD: The Williams & Wilkins Company, pp. 415–52.Google Scholar
Geist, V. (1971). Mountain Sheep: A Study in Behavior and Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Geist, V. (1987). On speciation in ice age mammals, with special reference to cervids and caprids. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 65: 1067–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, D., Bradshaw, J. W. S. & Wickens, S. M. (1997). Paedomorphosis affects agonistic visual signals of domestic dogs. Animal Behaviour, 53: 297–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1930). The Causes of Evolution. London: Longmans Green.Google Scholar
Hall, N., Lord, K., Arnold, A., Udell, M. & Wynne, C. (2015). Assessment of attachment behaviour to human caregivers in wolf pups (Canis lupus lupus). Behavioural Processes, 110: 15–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirobe, T. (1995). Structure and function of melanocytes: microscopic morphology and cell biology of mouse melanocytes in the epidermis and hair follicle. Histology and Histopathy, 10, 223–37.Google ScholarPubMed
Hole, F. & Wyllie, C. (2007). The oldest depictions of canines and a possible early breed of dog in Iran. Paléorient, 33: 175–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, M. (1988). The Dogs of Capitalism; Book 1: Origins. Austin, TX: Twenty First Century Logic.Google Scholar
Lantis, M. (1980). Changes in the Alaskan Eskimo relation of man to dog and their effect on two human diseases. Arctic Anthropology, 17: 2–24.Google Scholar
Larson, G. (2011). Genetics and domestication: important questions for new answers. Current Anthropology, 52: S485–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, G., Karlsson, E. K, Perri, A. et al. (2012). Rethinking dog domestication by integrating genetics archeology, and biogeography. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109: 8878–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Douarin, N. M. & Dupin, E. (1993). Cell lineage analysis in neural crest ontogeny. Journal of Neurobiology, 24: 146–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Le Lièvre, C. S. & Le Douarin, N. M. (1975). Mesenchymal derivatives of the neural crest: analysis of chimaeric quail and chick embryos. Journal of Embryology & Experimental Morphology. 34: 125–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Le Quellec, J. L. (2006). Rock art and cultural responses to climatic changes in the Central Sahara during the Holocene. In Exploring the Mind of Ancient Man, ed. Reddy, P. C.. New Delhi: Research India Press, pp. 173–88.Google Scholar
Levy, D. M. (1934). Experiments on the sucking reflex and social behavior of dogs. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 4: 203–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, K. (2010). A heterochronic explanation for the behaviorally polymorphic genus Canis: a study of the development of behavioral differences in dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and wolves (Canis lupus lupus) (Doctoral Dissertation). Retrieved from Electronic Doctoral Dissertations for Umass Amherst. Paper AAI3409623.
Lord, K. (2013). A comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Ethology. 119: 110–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, K., Feinstein, M. & Coppinger, R. (2009). Barking and mobbing. Behavioural Processes, 81: 358–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, K., Feinstein, M., Smith, B. & Coppinger, R. (2013). Variation in reproductive traits of members of the genus Canis with special attention to the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Behavioural Processes, 92: 131–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Løvtrup, S. (1977). The Phylogeny of Vertebrata. London: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Løvtrup, S. (1987). Darwinism: The Refutation of a Myth. London: Croom Helm.Google Scholar
MacRury, I. K. (1991). The Inuit Dog: its provenance, environment and history. Unpublished M.Phil. Thesis, Scott Polar Research Institute, University of Cambridge.
Miklósi, A. (2007). Dog Behaviour, Evolution, and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, M., Ottobre, J., Ottobre, A. et al. (2015). Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behavior in puppies. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 10: 286–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muro, C., Escobedo, R., Spector, L. & Coppinger, R. P. (2011). Wolf-pack (Canis lupus) hunting strategies emerge from simple rules in computational simulations. Behavioural Processes, 88: 192–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noden, D. M. (1978). The control of avian cephalic neural crest cytodifferentiation. I. skeletal and connective tissues. Developmental Biology. 67: 296–312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noden, D. M. (1983). The embryonic origins of avian cephalic and cervical muscles and associated connective tissues. The American Journal of Anatomy, 168: 257–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noden, D. M. & Schneider, R. A. (2006). Neural crest cells and the community of plan for craniofacial development: historical debates and current perspectives. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology. 589: 1–23.Google ScholarPubMed
Phillips, C. J., Coppinger, R. P. & Schimel, D. S. (1981). Hyperthermia in running sled dogs. Journal of Applied Physiology, 51: 135–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, E.O. (1984) Behavioral aspects of domestication. Quarterly Review of Biology. 59, 1–32 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawles, M. E. (1948). Origin of melanophores and their role in the development of color pattern in vertebrates. Physiological Reviews, 28: 383–408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rheingold, H. L. (1963). Maternal behavior in the dog. In Maternal Behavior in Mammals, ed. Rheingold, H. L.. NewYork: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., pp. 169–202.Google Scholar
Richardson, M. K. & Sieber-Blum, M. (1993). Pluripotent neural crest cells in the developing skin of the quail embryo. Developmental Biology, 157: 348–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenblatt, J. S. (1983). Olfaction mediates developmental transitions in the newborn of selected species of mammals. Journal of Neurobiology, 14: 347–75.Google Scholar
Sánchez-Villagra, M. R., Geiger, M. & Schneider, R. A. (2015). The taming of the neural crest: a developmental perspective on the origins of morphological covariation in domesticated mammals. Royal Society Open Science, 3: 160107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sands, M. W., Coppinger, R. P. & Phillips, C. J. (1977). Comparisons of thermal sweating and histology of sweat glands of selected canids. Journal of Mammalogy, 58: 74–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. A. (1999). Neural crest can form cartilages normally derived from mesoderm during development of the avian head skeleton. Developmental Biology, 208: 441–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. A. (2005). Developmental mechanisms facilitating the evolution of bills and quills. Journal of Anatomy, 207: 563–573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. A. (2015). Regulation of jaw length during development, disease, and evolution. Current Topics in Developmental Biology, 115: 271–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Schneider, R. A. & Helms, J. A. (2003). The cellular and molecular origins of beak morphology. Science, 299: 565–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P. (1963). The process of socialization in canine and human infants. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 28: 1–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. & Marston, M. (1950). Critical periods affecting the development of normal and mal-adjustive social behavior of puppies. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 77: 25–60.Google ScholarPubMed
Stanley, W. C. & Elliot, O. (1962). Differential human handling as reinforcing events and as treatments influencing later social behavior in basenji puppies. Psychological Reports, 10: 775–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stebbins, G. L. (1959). The role of hybridization in evolution. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 103: 231–51.Google Scholar
Stockard, C. R. (1941). The genetic and endocrinic basis for differences in form and behavior. American Anatomical Memoirs, 19: Philadelphia, PA: Wistar Institute of Anatomy and Biology.Google Scholar
Twitty, V. C. (1966). Of Scientists and Salamanders. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Udell, M., Lord, K., Feuerbacher, E. & Wynne, C. (2014). What is a human from a dog's perspective. In Dog Behavior and Cognition, ed. Horowitz, A., New York, NY: Springer, pp. 221–40.Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957). The Strategy of the Genes. London: George Allan and Unwin.Google Scholar
Webb, J. F. & Noden, D. M. (1993). Ectodermal Placodes: Contributions to the development of the vertebrate head. American Zoologist, 33: 434–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). The “domestication syndrome” in mammals: a unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics, 197: 795–808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woolpy, J. H. & Ginsburg, B. E. (1967). Wolf socialization: a study of temperament in a wild social species. American Zoologist, 7: 357–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimen, E. (1987). Ontogeny of approach and flight behavior toward humans in wolves, poodles and wolf-poodle hybrids. In Man and Wolf, ed. Frank, H.. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Dr. W. Junk Publishers, pp. 275–92.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Evolution of working dogs
    • By Kathryn Lord, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, MA, USA, Richard A. Schneider, Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, University of California at San Francisco, CA, USA, Raymond Coppinger, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, MA, USA
  • Edited by James Serpell, University of Pennsylvania
  • Book: The Domestic Dog
  • Online publication: 30 December 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781139161800.004
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Evolution of working dogs
    • By Kathryn Lord, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, MA, USA, Richard A. Schneider, Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, University of California at San Francisco, CA, USA, Raymond Coppinger, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, MA, USA
  • Edited by James Serpell, University of Pennsylvania
  • Book: The Domestic Dog
  • Online publication: 30 December 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781139161800.004
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Evolution of working dogs
    • By Kathryn Lord, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, Amherst, MA, USA, Richard A. Schneider, Department of Orthopaedic Surgery, University of California at San Francisco, CA, USA, Raymond Coppinger, School of Cognitive Science, Hampshire College, MA, USA
  • Edited by James Serpell, University of Pennsylvania
  • Book: The Domestic Dog
  • Online publication: 30 December 2016
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781139161800.004
Available formats
×