from PART IV - LIFE ON THE MARGINS
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 December 2016
Origins and evolution
One of the disconcerting ironies of science is that often the more we discover about a particular topic the less we seem to know about it. In the concluding chapter of the first edition of this book, I reported confidently that the dog was domesticated from the grey wolf (Canis lupus) at least 12 000 years ago, and that future discoveries would probably reveal precisely when, where and how this happened (Serpell, 1995). Despite the passage of 20 years, however, and the many remarkable new advances in archaeology and molecular genetics reviewed in this new edition (see Clutton-Brock, Chapter 2; vonHoldt & Driscoll, Chapter 3), the scientific debate concerning the origin(s) of the dog continues to rage without any clear consensus emerging as to the most likely time and place of domestication, whether it occurred only once or multiple times, or even the reasons why our ancestors decided to share their lives with this large and potentially dangerous carnivore in the first place (Drake et al., 2015; Larson et al., 2012; Skoglund et al., 2015; Thalman et al., 2013). Whether the discovery of new archaeological remains, or further advances in morphometrics and/or genetic sequencing will eventually settle these questions still remains to be seen.
Widely accepted ideas regarding the selective processes that led to the emergence of the domestic dog from its ancestor(s) may also need revision. Twenty years ago, Coppinger & Schneider (1995) made a compelling case that much of the variation in the working behavior and head shape of dogs was a coordinated product of different levels of selection for juvenile or puppy-like aspects of behavior (neoteny/pedomorphosis), an argument that seemed to resonate with the canine science community at the time. Now, these authors, together with Kathryn Lord, propose a decidedly different view; namely, that breed-specific dog behavior patterns, the shapes of their faces and jaws, and even the quality and color of their coats are all regulated by a population of embryonic cells known as “neural crest cells,” and that these cells act, “as a conduit through which breed-specific adaptations are implemented” (Lord et al., Chapter 4).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.