Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T13:49:42.722Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Becoming a dog: Early experience and the development of behavior

from PART II - BEHAVIOR, COGNITION AND TRAINING

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 December 2016

James Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Deborah L. Duffy
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Studies, School of Veterinary Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia PA, USA
J. Andrew Jagoe
Affiliation:
Abington Park Veterinary Group, Northampton, UK
James Serpell
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Early life experiences are known to have more profound and persistent effects on behavioral development than those occurring at other stages of the life cycle (see Bale et al., 2010; Bateson, 1981; Crews, 2011; Levine, 1962; Simmel & Baker, 1980). Pioneering studies of the domestic dog have contributed significantly to our current understanding of these effects of early experience. In 1945 an extensive program of research was initiated at the Roscoe B. Jackson Memorial Laboratory at Bar Harbor, Maine, on the relationship between heredity and social behavior in dogs. This work, which included detailed descriptive and experimental studies of behavioral genetics and ontogeny, led to the conclusion that there are particular “critical” periods in early development when puppies are unusually sensitive to environmental influences and, therefore, especially susceptible to the effects of early experience (see Elliot & Scott, 1961; Freedman et al., 1961; Fuller, 1967; Scott, 1962, 1963; Scott & Fuller, 1965; Scott & Marston, 1950; Scott et al., 1974).

In the last 10–15 years, the emerging field of behavioral epigenetics has revolutionized our understanding of the ways in which genes and experience – nature and nurture – interact during development to produce such effects. It is now known, for example, that a wide variety of environmental factors, including maternal behavior, physical and emotional stress, and exposure to toxins, drugs and hormones – especially during the early part of life – can modify the expression of the genes that regulate the central nervous system without altering the DNA sequence. This essentially means that, while the genome still provides the design blueprint for the developing brain and nervous system, it is an adjustable blueprint that can respond dynamically to information and experience coming from the environment, and thereby alter the course of behavioral development in adaptive ways (Bale et al., 2010; Berger et al., 2009; Cardoso et al., 2015; Heim & Binder, 2012; Sweatt, 2013; van den Berg, Chapter 5). Furthermore, at least some of these changes in gene expression appear to be heritable in the sense that they can be propagated, either through germ cells or via the quality of parental behavior, to affect later generations (Crews, 2011; Curley et al., 2011a, 2011b; Dunn et al., 2011; Lynch & Kemp, 2014; Patchev et al., 2014).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Domestic Dog
Its Evolution, Behavior and Interactions with People
, pp. 93 - 117
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, G. J. & Johnson, K. G. (1993). Sleep-wake cycles and other night-time behaviors of the domestic dog, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 36: 233–48.Google Scholar
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, C. S., Waters, E. & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment: A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Appleby, D. L., Bradshaw, J. W. S. & Casey, R. A. (2002). Relationship between aggressive and avoidance behavior by dogs and their experience in the first six months of life. Veterinary Record, 150: 434–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arueti, M., Perach-Barzilay, N., Tsoory, M. M., Berger, B., Getter, N. & Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2014) When two become one: the role of oxytocin in interpersonal coordination and cooperation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25: 1418–27.Google Scholar
Bale, T. L., Baram, T. Z., Brown, A. S. et al. (2010). Early life programming and neurodevelopmental disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 68: 314–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bales, K. L., Boone, E., Epperson, P., Hoffman, G. & Carter, C. S. (2011). Are behavioral effects of early experience mediated by oxytocin? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2: article 24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, W. B., Ayres, L. A., Gardner, C. L. & Foster, J. W. (1991). Den site activity patterns of grey wolves, Canis lupus, in southcentral Alaska. Canadian Field-Naturalist, 105, 497–504.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Animal Behavior, 27: 470–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P. (1981). Control of sensitivity to the environment during development. In Behavioral Development, ed. Immelmann, K., Barlow, G. W., Petrovich, L. & Main, M.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 433–53.Google Scholar
Bekoff, M. (1974). Social play and play-soliciting by infant canids. American Zoologist, 14, 323–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. (2012). Social dominance is not a myth: wolves, dogs, and other animals. Psychology Today, February 15. [Online]. Available: www.psychologytoday.com/blog/animal-emotions/201202/social-dominance-is-not-myth-wolves-dogs-and-other-animals
Belyaev, D. K., Plyusnina, I. Z. & Trut, L. N. (1985). Domestication in the silver fox (Vulpes vulpes): changes in physiological boundaries of the sensitive period of primary socialization. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 32: 253–68.Google Scholar
Berger, S. L., Kouzarides, T., shiekhattar, R. & Shilatifard, A. (2009). An operational definition of epigenetics. Genes and Development, 23: 781–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, E. J., Bradshaw, J. W. S. & Casey, R. A. (2013). Fear responses to noises in domestic dogs: prevalence, risk factors and co-occurrence with other fear related behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 145: 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borchelt, P. L. (1983a). Aggressive behavior of dogs kept as companion animals: classification and influence of sex, reproductive status and breed. Applied Animal Ethology, 10, 45–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borchelt, P. L. (1983b). Separation-elicited behavior problems in dogs. In New Perspectives on Our Lives with Companion Animals, eds. Katcher, A. H. & Beck, A. M.. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press, pp. 187–96.Google Scholar
Borchelt, P. L. (1984). Behavioral development of the puppy. In Nutrition and Behavior in Dogs and Cats, ed. Anderson, R. S.. Oxford: Pergamon Press, pp. 165–74.Google Scholar
Borchelt, P. L. & Voith, V. L. (1982a). Classification of animal behavior problems. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 12, 571–85.Google ScholarPubMed
Borchelt, P. L. & Voith, V. L. (1982b). Diagnosis and treatment of separation-related behavior problems in dogs. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 12, 625–35.Google ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and Loss, Vol. 2: Separation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Braastad, B.O., Osadchuk, L. V., Lund, G. & Bakken, M. (1998). Effects of prenatal handling stress on adrenal weight and behavior in novel situations in blue fox cubs (Alopex lagopus). Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 57: 157–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradshaw, J. W. S. & Nott, H. M. R. (1995). Social and communication behaviour of companion dogs. In The Domestic dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People, ed. Serpell, J. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cafazzo, S., Valsecchi, P., Bonanni, R. & Natoli, E. (2010). Dominance in relation to age, sex, and competitive contexts in a group of free-ranging domestic dogs. Behavioral Ecology, http://doi:10.1093/beheco/arq001 CrossRef
Campbell, W. E. (1975). Behavior Problems in Dogs. Santa Barbara, CA: American Veterinary Publications.Google ScholarPubMed
Campbell, W. E. (1986). The prevalence of behavioral problems in American dogs. Modern Veterinary Practice, 67: 28–31.Google Scholar
Cardoso, S. D., Teles, M. C. & Oliveira, R. F. (2015). Neurogenomic mechanisms of social plasticity. Journal of Experimental Biology, 218: 140–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, F. A. (2008). Epigenetic mechanisms and the transgenerational effects of maternal care. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 29: 386–97.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, F. A., Francis, D. D., Mar, A., Meaney, M. J. (2003). Variations in maternal care in the rat as a mediating influence for the effects of environment on development. Physiology and Behavior, 79: 359–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F. S., Barth, M. E., Johnson, S. L., Gotlib, I. H. & Johnson, S. C. (2011). Oxytocin receptor polymorphisms and attachment in human infants. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. http://doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00200 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clarke, R. S., Heron, W., Fetherstonhaugh, M. L., Forgays, D. G. & Hebb, D. O. (1951). Individual differences in dogs: preliminary report on the effects of early experience. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 5: 150–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cornwell, A. C. & Fuller, J. L. (1960). Conditioned responses in young puppies. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54: 13–15.Google Scholar
Crews, D. (2011). Epigenetic modifications of brain and behavior: theory and practice. Hormones and Behavior, 59: 393–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curley, J. P., Jensen, C. L., Mashoodh, R. & Champagne, F. A. (2011a). Social influences on neurobiology and behavior: epigenetic effects during development. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36: 352–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curley, J. P., Mashoodh, R. & Champagne, F. A. (2011b). Epigenetics and the origins of paternal effects. Hormones and Behavior, 59: 306–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeFries, J. C., Weir, M. W. & Hegmann, J. P. (1967). Differential effects of prenatal maternal stress on offspring behavior in mice as a function of genotype and stress. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63: 332–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehasse, J. (1994). Sensory, emotional and social development of the young dog. Bulletin for Veterinary Clinical Ethology, 2: 6–29.Google Scholar
Denenberg, V. H. (1968). A consideration of the usefulness of the critical period hypothesis as applied to the stimulation of rodents in infancy. In Early Experience and Behavior, eds. Newton, G. & Levine, S.. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas., pp. 142–67.Google Scholar
Denenberg, V. H. & Morton, J. R. C. (1962). Effects of environmental complexity and social groupings upon modification of emotional behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55: 242–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dorey, N. R., Udell, M. A. R. & Wynne, C. D. L. (2010). When do domestic dogs, Canis familiaris, start to understand human pointing? The role of ontogeny in the development of interspecies communication. Animal Behaviour, 79: 37–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, D. L., Hsu, Y. & Serpell, J. A. (2008). Breed differences in canine aggression. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 114: 441–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duffy, D. L. & Serpell, J. A. (2012). Predictive validity of a method for evaluating temperament in young guide and service dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 138: 99–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, G. A., Morgan, C. P. & Bale, T. L. (2011). Sex-specificity in transgenerational epigenetic programming. Hormones and Behavior, 59: 290–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykman, R. A., Mack, R. L. & Ackerman, P. T. (1965). The evaluation of autonomic and motor components of the nonavoidance conditioned response in the dog. Psychophysiology, 1, 209–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykman, R. A., Murphree, O. D. & Ackerman, P. T. (1966). Litter patterns in the offspring of nervous and stable dogs. II. Autonomic and motor conditioning. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 141: 419–31.Google Scholar
Dykman, R. A., Murphree, O. D. & Reese, W. G. (1979). Familial anthropophobia in pointer dogs? Archives of General Psychiatry, 36: 988–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliot, O. & Scott, J. P. (1961). The development of emotional distress reactions to separation in puppies. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 99: 3–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fentress, J. C. (1967). Observations on the behavioral development of a hand-reared male timber wolf. American Zoologist, 7, 339–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannigan, G. & Dodman, N. H. (2001). Risk factors and behaviors associated with separation anxiety in dogs. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 219: 460–466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, M. W. (1969). Behavioral effects of rearing dogs with cats during the “critical period of socialization”. Behavior, 35: 273–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1971). Behavior of Wolves, Dogs and Related Canids. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1972). Socio-ecological implications of individual differences in wolf litters: a developmental and evolutionary perspective. Behavior, 41: 298–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. (1978). The Dog: Its Domestication and Behavior. New York: Garland STPM Press.Google Scholar
Fox, M. W., Halperin, S., Wise, A. & Kohn, E. (1976). Species and hybrid differences in frequencies of play and agonistic actions in canids. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 40: 194–209.Google ScholarPubMed
Fox, M. W. & Stelzner, D. (1966). Behavioral effects of differential early experience in the dog. Animal Behavior, 14: 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foyer, P., Bjällerhag, N., Wilsson, W. & Jensen, P. (2014). Behaviour and experiences of dogs during the first year of life predict the outcome in a later temperament test. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 155: 93–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foyer, P., Wilsson, E., Wright, D. & Jensen, P. (2013). Early experiences modulate stress coping in a population of German shepherd dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 146: 79–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, D. D., Diorio, J., Plotsky, P. M. & Meaney, M. J. (2002). Environmental enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress reactivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 22: 7840–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, H. & Frank, M. G. (1982). On the effects of domestication on canine social development and behavior. Applied Animal Ethology, 8: 507–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, H. & Frank, M. G. (1985). Comparative manipulation-test performance in ten-week-old wolves (Canis lupus) and Alaskan malamutes (Canis familiaris): a Piagetian interpretation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99: 266–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freedman, D. G., King, J. A. & Elliot, O. (1961). Critical periods in the social development of dogs. Science, 133: 1016–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuller, J. L. (1967). Experiential deprivation and later behavior. Science, 158: 1645–52.Google ScholarPubMed
Gácsi, M., Kara, E., Belényi, B. & Topál, J. (2009). The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs. Animal Cognition, 12: 471–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gagnon, S. & Doré, F. Y. (1994). Cross-sectional study of object permanence in domestic puppies (Canis familiaris). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108: 226–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzano, A., Mariti, C., Notari, L., Sighieri, C. & McBride, E. A. (2008). Effects of early gentling and early environment on emotional development of puppies. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 110: 294–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goddard, M. E. & Beilharz, R. G. (1982). Genetic and environmental factors affecting the suitability of dogs as guide dogs for the blind. Theoretical and Applied Genetics, 62, 97–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goddard, M. E. & Beilharz, R. G. (1984). Factor analysis of fearfulness in potential guide dogs. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 12, 253–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976). The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In Neural and Behavioral Specificity, ed. Scott, J. P.. New York: Academic Press, pp. 25–54.Google Scholar
Gray, D. R. (1993). The use of muskox kill sites as temporary rendezvous sites by arctic wolves with pups in early winter. Arctic, 46: 324–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haase, E. (2000). Comparison of reproductive biological parameters in male wolves and domestic dogs. Zeitschrift fur Saugetierkunde, 65: 257–70.Google Scholar
Harlow, H. F. & Harlow, M. K. (1966). Learning to love. American Scientist, 54: 244–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Hart, B. L., Hart, L. A. & Bain, M. J. (2006). Canine and Feline Behavioral Therapy; edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harvey, N. D., Craigon, P. J., Blythe, S. A., England, G. C. W. & Asher, L.(2016). Social rearing environment influences dog behavioral development. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2016.03.004.CrossRef
Hayssen, V., van Tienhoven, A. & van Tienhoven, A. (1993). Asdell's Patterns of Mammalian Reproduction. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Heim, C. & Binder, E. B. (2012) Current research trends in early life stress and depression: Review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene-environment interactions, and epigenetics. Experimental Neurology, 233: 102–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hepper, P. G. (1985). Sibling recognition in the domestic dog. Animal Behaviour, 34: 288–9.Google Scholar
Herron, M. E., Lord, L. K., Hill, L. N. & Reisner, I. R. (2007). Effects of preadoption counseling for owners on house-training success among dogs acquired from shelters. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 231: 558–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, K., Skibiel, A. L., Foster, A. B. et al. (2014). Cortisol in mother's milk across lactation reflects maternal life history and predicts infant temperament. Behavioral Ecology: http://doi:10.1093/beheco/aru186 CrossRef
Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behavior, edition. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1974). Biological Bases of Human Social Behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Immelmann, K. and Soumi, S. J. (1981). Sensitive phases in development. In Behavioral Development, eds. Immelmann, K., Barlow, G. W., Petrovich, L. & Main, M.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 395–431.Google Scholar
Ito, A., Kikusui, T., Takeuchi, Y. & Mori, Y. (2006), Effects of early weaning on anxiety and autonomic responses to stress in rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 171: 87–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jagoe, J. A. (1994). Behavior problems in the domestic dog: a retrospective and prospective study to identify factors influencing their development. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of Cambridge.
Joffe, J. M. (1969). Prenatal Determinants of Behavior. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Kappeler, L. & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Epigenetics and parental effects. Bioessays 32: 818–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kikusui, T., Nakamura, K., Kakuma, Y. & Mori, Y. (2006). Early weaning augments neuroendocrine stress responses in mice. Behavioural Brain Research, 175: 96–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kis, A., Bence, M., Lakatos, G. et al. (2014). Oxytocin gene receptor polymorphisms are associated with human directed social behavior in dogs (Canis familiaris). PloS One, 9: e83993. http://doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083993 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leon, M. (1992). Neuroethology of olfactory preference development. Journal of Neurobiology, 23: 1557–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. (1962). Plasma free corticosteroid response to electric shock in rats stimulated in infancy. Science, 135: 795–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, S. (1967). Maternal and environmental influences on the adrenocortical response to stress in weanling rats. Science, 156; 258–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindsay, S. R. (2001). Handbook of Applied Dog Behavior and Training, Vol. 2: Etiology and Assessment of Behavior Problems. Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Line, S. & Voith, V. L. (1986). Dominance aggression of dogs towards people: behavior profile and response to treatment. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 16: 77–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, R. (1979). Dominance in wolves: useful construct or bad habit? In The Behavior and Ecology of Wolves, ed. Klinghammer, E.. New York: Garland STPM Press, pp. 225–44.Google Scholar
Lord, K. (2013). Comparison of the sensory development of wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). Ethology, 119: 110–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. Journal für Ornithologie, 83: 137–213, 289–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966). On Aggression. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R. & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behavior and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10: 434–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lynch, K. E. & Kemp, D. J. (2014). Nature-via-nurture and unraveling causality in evolutionary genetics. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.10.005 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, D. M., Parker, K. J. & Schatzberg, A. F. (2010). Animal models of early life stress: implications for understanding resilience. Developmental Psychobiology, 52: 616–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, K. B. (1983). Stability of individual differences in behavior in a litter of wolf, cubs (Canis lupus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97: 99–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, K. B. (1987). Development and stability of personality characteristics in pre-pubertal wolves: implications for pack organization and behavior. In Man and Wolf, ed. Frank, H.. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr W. Junk Publishers, pp. 293–312.Google Scholar
Macrì, S., Zoratto, F. & Laviola, G. (2011). Early-stress regulates resilience, vulnerability and experimental validity in laboratory rodents through mother-offspring hormonal transfer. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 35: 1534–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malm, K. (1995). Behaviour of parents and offspring in two canids. Unpublished Ph.D., Skara, Sweden: Sveriges Lantbruksuniversitet.
McCrave, E. A. (1991). Diagnostic criteria for separation anxiety in the dog. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 21: 247–55.Google ScholarPubMed
McGreevy, P. D., Georgevsky, D., Carrasco, J., Valenzuela, M., Duffy, D. L. & Serpell, J. A. (2013). Dog behavior co-varies with height, bodyweight and skull shape. PLoS ONE 8: e80529. http://doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080529 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMillan, F. D., Serpell, J. A., Duffy, D. L., Masaoud, E. & Dohoo, I. R. (2013). Differences in behavioral and psychological characteristics between dogs obtained as puppies from pet stores and those obtained from noncommercial breeders. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 242: 1359–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2001). Maternal care, gene expression, and the transmission of individual differences in stress reactivity across generations. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24: 1161–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mech, L. D. (1970). The Wolf: the Ecology and Behavior of an Endangered Species. New York: Natural History Press.Google Scholar
Mech, L. D. (2008). Whatever happened to the term “alpha wolf’? International Wolf, Winter: 4–8: www.wolf.org/
Mech, L. D. & Cluff, H. D. (2010). Prolonged intensive dominance behavior between grey wolves, Canis lupus. Canadian Field-Naturalist, 124: 215–18.Google Scholar
Melzack, R. (1954). The genesis of emotional behavior: an experimental study of the dog. Journal of comparative and physiological Psychology, 47; 166–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melzack, R. & Thompson, W. R. (1956). Effects of early experience on social behavior. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10: 82–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, R. M. (2014). Sensitive and critical periods during neurotypical and aberrant neurodevelopment: a framework for neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.12.001 CrossRef
Miklósi, A. (2007). Dog Behaviour. Evolution, and Cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, D. M., Stats, S. R., Partlo, B. S. & Rada, K. (1996). Factors associated with the decision to surrender a pet to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209: 738–42.Google Scholar
Morrow, M., Ottobre, J., Ottobre, A. et al. (2015). Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behavior in puppies. Journal of Veterinary Behavior. http://doi:10.1016/j.jveb.2015.03.002 CrossRef
Moyer, K. E. (1968). Kinds of aggression and their physiological basis. Communications in Behavioral Biology. Part A, 2, 65–87.Google Scholar
Mugford, R. A. (1985). Attachment versus dominance: an alternative view of the man-dog relationship. In The Human-Pet Relationship. Vienna: IEMT (Institute for Interdisciplinary Research on the Human-Pet Relationship), pp. 157–65.Google Scholar
Murphree, O. D. (1973). Inheritance of human aversion and inactivity in two strains of the pointer dog. Biological Psychiatry, 7: 23–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Murphree, O. D., Angel, C, DeLuca, D. C. & Newton, J. E. O. (1977). Longitudinal studies of genetically nervous dogs. Biological Psychiatry, 12: 573–6.Google ScholarPubMed
Murphree, O. D. & Dykman, R. A. (1965). Litter patterns in the offspring of nervous and stable dogs. I. Behavioral tests. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 141: 321–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nephew, B. & Murgatroyd, C. (2013). The role of maternal care in shaping CNS function. Neuropeptides, 47: 371–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newton, J. E. O. & Lucas, L. A. (1982). Differential heart-rate responses to person in nervous and normal pointer dogs. Behavioral Genetics, 12, 379–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niebuhr, B. R., Levinson, M., Nobbe, D. E. & Tiller, J. E. (1980). Treatment of an incompletely socialized dog. In Canine Behavior, ed. Hart, B. L.. Santa Barbara, CA: Veterinary Practice Publishing Co., p. 83.Google Scholar
O'Farrell, V. (1986). Manual of Canine Behavior. Cheltenham: BSAVA Publications.Google Scholar
Overall, K. L. (1997). Clinical Behavioral Medicine for Small Animals. St. Louis, MO: Mosby-Year Book, Inc. Google Scholar
Pal, S. K. (2005) Parental care in free-ranging dogs, Canis familiaris. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 90: 31–47.Google Scholar
Pal, S. K. (2008). Maturation and development of social behavior during early ontogeny in free-ranging dog puppies in West Bengal, India. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 111: 95–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patchev, A. V., Rodrigues, A. J., Sousa, N., Spengler, D. & Almeida, O. F. X. (2014). The future is now: Early life events preset adult behaviour. Acta Physiologica, 210: 46–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patronek, G. J., Glickman, L. T., Beck, A. M., McCabe, G. P. & Ecker, C. (1996). Risk factors for relinquishment of dogs to an animal shelter. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association, 209: 572–81.Google Scholar
Pedersen, V. & Jeppesen, L. L. (1990). Effects of early handling on later behavior and stress responses in the Silver Fox (Vulpes vulpes). Applied Animal Behavior Science, 26: 383–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfaffenberger, C. J. & Scott, J. P. (1976). Early rearing and testing. In Guide Dogs for the Blind: Their Selection, Development and Training, eds. Pfaffenberger, C. J., Scott, J. P., Fuller, J. L., Ginsburg, B. E. & Bielfelt, S. W.. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 13–37.Google Scholar
Pfaffenberger, C. J., Scott, J. P., Fuller, J. L., Ginsburg, B. E. & Bielfelt, S. W. (1976). Guide Dogs for the Blind: Their Selection, Development and Training. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Pierantoni, L., Albertini, M. and Pirrone, F. (2011). Prevalence of owner-reported behaviours in dogs separated from the litter at two different ages. Veterinary Record, 169: 648 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pluijmakers, J. J. T. M., Appleby, D. L. & Bradshaw, J. W. S. (2010). Exposure to video images between 3 and 5 weeks of age decreases neophobia in domestic dogs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 126: 51–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plyusnina, I. Z., Oskina, I. N. & Trut, L. N. (1991). An analysis of fear and aggression during early development of behavior in silver foxes (Vulpes vulpes). Applied Animal Behavior Science, 32: 253–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podberscek, A. L., Hsu, Y. & Serpell, J. A. (1999). Evaluation of clomipramine as an adjunct to behavioural therapy in the treatment of separation-related problems in dogs. Veterinary Record, 145: 365–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, E. O. (1984). Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Quarterly Review of Biology, 59: 1–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riedel, J., Schumann, K., Kaminski, J., Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (2008). The early ontogeny of dog-human communication. Animal Behaviour, 75: 1003–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roll, A. & Unshelm, J. (1997). Aggressive conflicts among dogs and factors affecting them. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 52: 229–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salman, M. D., New, J. G., Scarlett, J. M., Kass, P. H., Ruch-Gallie, R. & Hetts, S. (1998). Human and animal factors related to the relinquishment of dogs and cats in 12 selected animal shelters in the United States. Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science, 1: 207–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Mothering style and methylation. Nature Neuroscience, 7: 791–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, M. V. (2010). Molecular mechanisms of early life stress – lessons from mouse models. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34: 845–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoon, A. & Berntsen, T. G. (2011). Evaluating the effect of early neurological stimulation on the development and training of mine detection dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 6: 150–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, J. P. (1962). Critical periods in behavioral development. Science, 138: 949–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P. (1963). The process of primary socialization in canine and human infants. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 28: 1–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P. & Bielfelt, S. W. (1976). Analysis of the puppy testing program. In Guide Dogs for the Blind: Their Selection, Development and Training, eds. Pfaffenberger, C. J., Scott, J. P., Fuller, J. L., Ginsburg, B. E. & Bielfelt, S. W.. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 39–75.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. & Fuller, J. L. (1965). Genetics and the Social Behavior of the Dog. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Scott, J. P. & Marston, M. V. (1950). Critical periods affecting the development of normal and mal-adjustive social behavior of puppies. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 77: 25–60.Google ScholarPubMed
Scott, J. P., Stewart, J. M. & DeGhett, V. J. (1974). Critical periods in the organization of systems. Developmental Psychobiology, 7: 489–513.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (1983). The personality of the dog and its influence on the pet-owner bond. In New Perspectives on Our Lives with Companion Animals, eds. Katcher, A. H. & Beck, A. M.. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press, pp. 57–71.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (1987). The influence of inheritance and environment on canine behavior: myth and fact. Journal of small Animal Practice, 28: 949–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, J. A. (2003). Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection – beyond the “cute response”. Society & Animals, 11: 83–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, J. A. & Duffy, D. L. (2014). Breeds and their behavior. In Domestic Dog Cognition and Behavior, ed. Horowitz, A.. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, pp. 31–57.Google Scholar
Serpell, J. A. & Duffy, D. L. (2016). Aspects of juvenile and adolescent environment predict aggression and fear in 12 month-old guide dogs. Frontiers in Veterinary Science, 3: 49. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2016.00049CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Serpell, J. A. & Jagoe, J. A. (1995). Early experience and the development of behavior. In The Domestic Dog: Its Evolution, Behaviour and Interactions with People, ed. Serpell, J. A.. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 80–102.Google Scholar
Sherman, B. L. & Mills, D. S. (2008). Canine anxieties and phobias: an update of separation anxiety and noise aversions. Veterinary Clinics Small Animal Practice, 38: 1081–106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shull-Selcer, E. A. & Stagg, W. (1991). Advances in the understanding and treatment of noise phobias. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 21: 353–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Simmel, E. C. & Baker, E. (1980). The effects of early experiences on later behavior: a critical discussion. In Early Experiences and Early Behavior: Implications for Social Development, ed. Simmell, E. C.. New York: Academic Press, pp. 3–13.Google Scholar
Slabbert, J. M. & Rasa, O. A. E. (1993). The effect of early separation from the mother on pups in bonding to humans and pup health. Journal of the South African Veterinary Association, 64: 4–8.Google Scholar
Smotherman, W. P., Brown, C. P. & Levine, S. (1977). Maternal responsiveness following differential pup treatment and mother-pup interactions. Hormones and Behavior, 8: 242–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, W. C. (1970). Feeding behavior and learning in neonatal dogs. In Second Symposium on Oral Sensation and Perception, ed. Bosma, J. F.. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas, pp. 242–90.Google Scholar
Stanley, W. C. (1972). Perspectives in behavior organization and development resulting from studies of feeding behavior in infant dogs. In Third Symposium on Oral Sensation and Perception: The Mouth of the Infant, ed. Bosma, J. F.. Springfield, IL: Charles Thomas, pp. 188–257.Google Scholar
Stur, I. (1987). Genetic aspects of temperament and behavior in dogs. Journal of Small Animal Practice, 28: 957–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweatt, D. J. (2013). The emerging field of neuroepigenetics. Neuron, 80: 624–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thorne, F. C. (1944). The inheritance of shyness in dogs. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 65: 275–9.Google Scholar
Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Miklósi, A., Virányi, Z., Kubinyi, E. & Csányi, V. (2005). Attachment to humans: A comparative study on hand-reared wolves and differently socialized dog puppies. Animal Behaviour, 70: 1367–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tiira, K., Hakosalo, O., Kareinen, L. et al. (2012). Environmental effects on compulsive tail-chasing in dogs, PLoS One, 7: e41684. http://doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041684 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trut, L. N., Plyusnina, I.Z. & Oskina, I.N. (2004). An experiment on fox domestication and debatable issues of evolution of the dog. Russian Journal of Genetics, 40: 794–807.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuber, D. S., Hothersall, D. & Peters, M. F. (1982). Treatment of fears and phobias in dogs. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 12: 607–23.Google ScholarPubMed
van Hooff, J. A. R. A. M. & Wensing, J. A. B. (1987). Dominance and its behavioral measures in a captive wolf pack. In Man and Wolf, ed. Frank, H.. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr W. Junk Publishers, pp. 219–51.Google Scholar
Voith, V. L. & Borchelt, P. L. (1982). Diagnosis and treatment of dominance aggression in dogs. Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice, 12: 655–63.Google ScholarPubMed
Voith, V. L. & Borchelt, P. L. (1985a). Fears and phobias in dogs. Compendium of Continuing Education for the Practising Veterinarian, 7: 209–18.Google Scholar
Voith, V. L. & Borchelt, P. L. (1985b). Separation anxiety in dogs. Compendium on Continuing Education for the Practising Veterinarian, 7: 42–52.Google Scholar
Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A. et al. (2004) Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7: 847–854.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, D. L. & Hepper, P. G. (2006). Prenatal olfactory learning in the domestic dog. Animal Behaviour, 72: 681–686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinstock, M. (2008). Long-term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32: 1073–1086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whimbey, A. E. & Denenberg, V. H. (1967). Two independent behavioral dimensions in open-field performance. Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology, 63: 500–504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilsson, E. (1984). The social interaction between mother and offspring during weaning in German shepherd dogs: individual differences between mothers and their effects on offspring. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 13: 101–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfle, T. L. (1990). Policy, program and people: the three Ps to well-being. In Canine Research Environment, eds. Mench, J. A. & Krulisch, L.. Bethesda, MD: Scientists Center for Animal Welfare, pp. 41–7.Google Scholar
Woolpy, J. H. (1968). The social organisation of wolves. Natural History, 77: 46–55.Google Scholar
Woolpy, J. H. & Ginsburg, B. E. (1967). Wolf socialization: a study of temperament in a wild social species. American Zoologist, 7: 357–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, J. C. (1980). Early development of exploratory behavior and dominance in three litters of German shepherds. In Early Experiences and Early Behavior: Implications for Social Development, ed. Simmel, E. C.. New York: Academic Press, pp. 181–206.Google Scholar
Wright, J. C. & Nesselrote, M. S. (1987). Classification of behavior problems in dogs: distributions of age, breed, sex and reproductive status. Applied Animal Behavior Science, 19: 169–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynne, C. D. L., Udell, M. A. R. & Lord, K. A. (2008). Ontogeny's impacts on human-dog communication. Animal Behaviour, 76: e1–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zimen, E. (1987). Ontogeny of approach and flight behavior towards humans in wolves, poodles and wolf-poodle hybrids. In Man and Wolf, ed. Frank, H.. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Dr W. Junk Publishers, pp. 275–92.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×