from PART II - BEHAVIOR, COGNITION AND TRAINING
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 30 December 2016
Introduction
Early life experiences are known to have more profound and persistent effects on behavioral development than those occurring at other stages of the life cycle (see Bale et al., 2010; Bateson, 1981; Crews, 2011; Levine, 1962; Simmel & Baker, 1980). Pioneering studies of the domestic dog have contributed significantly to our current understanding of these effects of early experience. In 1945 an extensive program of research was initiated at the Roscoe B. Jackson Memorial Laboratory at Bar Harbor, Maine, on the relationship between heredity and social behavior in dogs. This work, which included detailed descriptive and experimental studies of behavioral genetics and ontogeny, led to the conclusion that there are particular “critical” periods in early development when puppies are unusually sensitive to environmental influences and, therefore, especially susceptible to the effects of early experience (see Elliot & Scott, 1961; Freedman et al., 1961; Fuller, 1967; Scott, 1962, 1963; Scott & Fuller, 1965; Scott & Marston, 1950; Scott et al., 1974).
In the last 10–15 years, the emerging field of behavioral epigenetics has revolutionized our understanding of the ways in which genes and experience – nature and nurture – interact during development to produce such effects. It is now known, for example, that a wide variety of environmental factors, including maternal behavior, physical and emotional stress, and exposure to toxins, drugs and hormones – especially during the early part of life – can modify the expression of the genes that regulate the central nervous system without altering the DNA sequence. This essentially means that, while the genome still provides the design blueprint for the developing brain and nervous system, it is an adjustable blueprint that can respond dynamically to information and experience coming from the environment, and thereby alter the course of behavioral development in adaptive ways (Bale et al., 2010; Berger et al., 2009; Cardoso et al., 2015; Heim & Binder, 2012; Sweatt, 2013; van den Berg, Chapter 5). Furthermore, at least some of these changes in gene expression appear to be heritable in the sense that they can be propagated, either through germ cells or via the quality of parental behavior, to affect later generations (Crews, 2011; Curley et al., 2011a, 2011b; Dunn et al., 2011; Lynch & Kemp, 2014; Patchev et al., 2014).
To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.
Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.
Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.
To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.